Integrated Neural Representations of Odor Intensity and Affective Valence in Human Amygdala

Arousal and valence are proposed to represent fundamental dimensions of emotion. The neural substrates for processing these aspects of stimuli are studied widely, with recent studies of chemosensory processing suggesting the amygdala processes intensity (a surrogate for arousal) rather than valence. However, these investigations have assumed that a valence effect in the amygdala is linear such that testing valence extremes is sufficient to infer responses across valence space. In this study, we tested an alternative hypothesis, namely that valence responses in the amygdala are nonlinear. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging, we measured amygdala responses to high- and low-concentration variants of pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant odors. Our results demonstrate that the amygdala exhibits an intensity-by-valence interaction in olfactory processing. In other words, the effect of intensity on amygdala activity is not the same at all levels of valence. Specifically, the amygdala responds differentially to high (vs low)-intensity odor for pleasant and unpleasant smells but not for neutral smells. This implies that the amygdala codes neither intensity nor valence per se, but a combination that we suggest reflects the overall emotional value of a stimulus.

[1]  R Turner,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[2]  J. D. McGaugh,et al.  Involvement of the amygdala in memory storage: interaction with other brain systems. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  S. Rauch,et al.  Masked Presentations of Emotional Facial Expressions Modulate Amygdala Activity without Explicit Knowledge , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  D. Zald The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli , 2003, Brain Research Reviews.

[5]  J. Pardo,et al.  Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[6]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  Involvement of right piriform cortex in olfactory familiarity judgments , 2005, NeuroImage.

[7]  Lennart Heimer,et al.  Chapter I - The human basal forebrain. Part I. An overview , 1999 .

[8]  Chantal Delon-Martin,et al.  fMRI of emotional responses to odors: influence of hedonic valence and judgment, handedness, and gender , 2003, NeuroImage.

[9]  S. Arctander,et al.  Perfume And Flavor Chemicals: (Aroma Chemicals) , 2000 .

[10]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Flavor processing: more than the sum of its parts , 1997, Neuroreport.

[11]  Stephan Hamann,et al.  Nosing in on the emotional brain , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[12]  S. Schiffman Physicochemical correlates of olfactory quality. , 1974, Science.

[13]  F. S. Hough Part I. An overview , 1987 .

[14]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Encoding Predictive Reward Value in Human Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex , 2003, Science.

[15]  B. Green,et al.  Derivation and evaluation of a semantic scale of oral sensation magnitude with apparent ratio properties , 1993 .

[16]  Anders Björklund,et al.  The primate nervous system , 1997 .

[17]  P. Lang The emotion probe. Studies of motivation and attention. , 1995, The American psychologist.

[18]  M. Mesulam,et al.  Dissociation of Neural Representation of Intensity and Affective Valuation in Human Gustation , 2003, Neuron.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[20]  J. Russell A circumplex model of affect. , 1980 .

[21]  Ralf Deichmann,et al.  Functional Heterogeneity in Human Olfactory Cortex: An Event-Related Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  R. J Dolan,et al.  Common and distinct neural responses during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions , 2003, NeuroImage.

[23]  M. Bensafi,et al.  Autonomic nervous system responses to odours: the role of pleasantness and arousal. , 2002, Chemical senses.

[24]  B Cowart,et al.  Evaluating the 'Labeled Magnitude Scale' for measuring sensations of taste and smell. , 1996, Chemical senses.

[25]  G. Glover,et al.  Dissociated neural representations of intensity and valence in human olfaction , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  Thomas J. Ross,et al.  Amygdala response to both positively and negatively valenced stimuli , 2001, Neuroreport.

[27]  Adam P. Morris,et al.  Amygdala Responses to Fearful and Happy Facial Expressions under Conditions of Binocular Suppression , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[29]  Noam Sobel,et al.  Dissociating Intensity from Valence as Sensory Inputs to Emotion , 2003, Neuron.

[30]  R. Dolan,et al.  Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala , 1998, Nature.

[31]  Florin Dolcos,et al.  Dissociable effects of arousal and valence on prefrontal activity indexing emotional evaluation and subsequent memory: an event-related fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[32]  S. Hamann,et al.  Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala , 2002, Neuroreport.

[33]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.