Cortical route for facelike pattern processing in human newborns

Significance Newborns show a remarkable ability to detect faces even minutes after birth, an ecologically fundamental skill that is instrumental for interacting with their conspecifics. What are the neural bases of this expertise? Using EEG and a slow oscillatory visual stimulation, we identified a reliable response specific to facelike patterns in newborns, for which underlying cortical sources overlap with the adult face-specific cortical circuit. This suggests that the development of face perception in infants might rely on an early cortical route specialized in face processing already shortly after birth. Humans are endowed with an exceptional ability for detecting faces, a competence that, in adults, is supported by a set of face-specific cortical patches. Human newborns, already shortly after birth, preferentially orient to faces, even when they are presented in the form of highly schematic geometrical patterns vs. perceptually equivalent nonfacelike stimuli. The neural substrates underlying this early preference are still largely unexplored. Is the adult face-specific cortical circuit already active at birth, or does its specialization develop slowly as a function of experience and/or maturation? We measured EEG responses in 1- to 4-day-old awake, attentive human newborns to schematic facelike patterns and nonfacelike control stimuli, visually presented with slow oscillatory “peekaboo” dynamics (0.8 Hz) in a frequency-tagging design. Despite the limited duration of newborns’ attention, reliable frequency-tagged responses could be estimated for each stimulus from the peak of the EEG power spectrum at the stimulation frequency. Upright facelike stimuli elicited a significantly stronger frequency-tagged response than inverted facelike controls in a large set of electrodes. Source reconstruction of the underlying cortical activity revealed the recruitment of a partially right-lateralized network comprising lateral occipitotemporal and medial parietal areas overlapping with the adult face-processing circuit. This result suggests that the cortical route specialized in face processing is already functional at birth.

[1]  C Umiltà,et al.  Preferential orienting to faces in newborns: a temporal-nasal asymmetry. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial? , 2002, Developmental psychology.

[3]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  A face-selective ventral occipito-temporal map of the human brain with intracerebral potentials , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[4]  J. Palva,et al.  Preterm Birth Changes Networks of Newborn Cortical Activity , 2019, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  J. Haxby,et al.  Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Contrast sensitivity of the human neonate measured by the visual evoked potential. , 1979, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[7]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  From faces to hands: Changing visual input in the first two years , 2016, Cognition.

[8]  F. Simion,et al.  Can a Nonspecific Bias Toward Top-Heavy Patterns Explain Newborns' Face Preference? , 2004, Psychological science.

[9]  Y. Sugita Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[10]  C. Jacques,et al.  The N170 : understanding the time-course of face perception in the human brain , 2011 .

[11]  M. Buiatti,et al.  Electrophysiological evidence of statistical learning of long-distance dependencies in 8-month-old preterm and full-term infants , 2015, Brain and Language.

[12]  S. Huffel,et al.  Relationship of EEG sources of neonatal seizures to acute perinatal brain lesions seen on MRI: A pilot study , 2013, Human brain mapping.

[13]  Joachim Gross,et al.  Good practice for conducting and reporting MEG research , 2013, NeuroImage.

[14]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Cortical specialisation for face processing: face-sensitive event-related potential components in 3- and 12-month-old infants , 2003, NeuroImage.

[15]  D. Maurer,et al.  Infants' perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. , 1981, Child development.

[16]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline , 1991, Cognition.

[17]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Difference in Visual Social Predispositions Between Newborns at Low- and High-risk for Autism , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[18]  Sampsa Vanhatalo,et al.  Early development of spatial patterns of power-law frequency scaling in FMRI resting-state and EEG data in the newborn brain. , 2013, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  Bruno Rossion,et al.  Figures and figure supplements , 2014 .

[20]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[21]  Arcangelo Merla,et al.  Infant cortex responds to other humans from shortly after birth , 2013, Scientific Reports.

[22]  G. Dehaene-Lambertz,et al.  Right but not left hemispheric discrimination of faces in infancy , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[23]  Marco Buiatti,et al.  Investigating the neural correlates of continuous speech computation with frequency-tagged neuroelectric responses , 2009, NeuroImage.

[24]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Oxford Handbook of Face Perception , 2011 .

[25]  Fernando Lopes da Silva,et al.  Biophysical Aspects of EEG and MEG , 2022 .

[26]  F. Turkheimer,et al.  Emergence of resting state networks in the preterm human brain , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Chia-Chun Hung,et al.  Functional Mapping of Face-Selective Regions in the Extrastriate Visual Cortex of the Marmoset , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  J. Haxby,et al.  Distributed Neural Systems for Face Perception , 2011 .

[29]  Daniel D. Dilks,et al.  Organization of high-level visual cortex in human infants , 2017, Nature Communications.

[30]  Mostefa Mesbah,et al.  Neonatal EEG at scalp is focal and implies high skull conductivity in realistic neonatal head models , 2014, NeuroImage.

[31]  Richard M. Leahy,et al.  Brainstorm: A User-Friendly Application for MEG/EEG Analysis , 2011, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[32]  C. C. Goren,et al.  Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. , 1975, Pediatrics.

[33]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Edward F. Ester,et al.  Substitution and pooling in visual crowding induced by similar and dissimilar distractors. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[35]  Giorgio Vallortigara,et al.  Core knowledge of object, number, and geometry: A comparative and neural approach , 2012, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[36]  Tomoki Arichi,et al.  Specialization and integration of functional thalamocortical connectivity in the human infant , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  C. Umilta,et al.  Face preference at birth. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[38]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[39]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Newborns' preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[40]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Development of human visual function , 2011, Vision Research.

[41]  G. F. Cooper,et al.  Development of the Brain depends on the Visual Environment , 1970, Nature.

[42]  Tim Donovan,et al.  The Human Fetus Preferentially Engages with Face-like Visual Stimuli , 2017, Current Biology.

[43]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Face-sensitive cortical processing in early infancy. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[44]  J. Atkinson,et al.  Orientation-specific cortical responses develop in early infancy , 1986, Nature.

[45]  Justin L. Vincent,et al.  Development of the macaque face-patch system , 2017, Nature Communications.

[46]  Stefanie Peykarjou,et al.  Three-Month-Olds’ Brain Responses to Upright and Inverted Faces and Cars , 2013, Developmental neuropsychology.

[47]  G. Vallortigara,et al.  Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: Early attachment in chicks and babies , 2017, Behavioural Brain Research.

[48]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a 'low road' to 'many roads' of evaluating biological significance , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[49]  Justin M. Ales,et al.  The steady-state visual evoked potential in vision research: A review. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[50]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[51]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Comparing face patch systems in macaques and humans , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[52]  Mark H. Johnson Subcortical face processing , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[53]  D. L. Schomer,et al.  Niedermeyer's Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields , 2012 .

[54]  Margaret S. Livingstone,et al.  Seeing faces is necessary for face-patch formation , 2017, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  J. Haxby,et al.  The distributed human neural system for face perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[56]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  The Evolution of Social Orienting: Evidence from Chicks (Gallus gallus) and Human Newborns , 2011, PloS one.

[57]  B. Mazoyer,et al.  Neural Correlates of Woman Face Processing by 2-Month-Old Infants , 2002, NeuroImage.

[58]  Anton Tokariev,et al.  Analysis of infant cortical synchrony is constrained by the number of recording electrodes and the recording montage , 2016, Clinical Neurophysiology.