Not My Protector—Women Have an Aversion to High Dark Triad Faces Irrespective of Childhood or Current Environmental Danger
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Ike E. Onyishi,et al. Women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions , 2019, Scientific Reports.
[2] P. Christiansen,et al. A drunk heart speaks a sober mind: Alcohol does not influence the selection of short-term partners with dark triad traits , 2017, Personality and Individual Differences.
[3] U. Ehlert,et al. The influence of income and testosterone on the validity of facial width-to-height ratio as a biomarker for dominance , 2018, PloS one.
[4] Virgil Zeigler‐Hill,et al. The fundamental social motives that characterize dark personality traits , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.
[5] L. DeBruine,et al. Are dark triad cues really visible in faces? , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.
[6] E. Bates,et al. How Dark Personalities Perpetrate Partner and General Aggression in Sweden and the United Kingdom , 2018, Journal of interpersonal violence.
[7] Gregory L. Carter,et al. Sensation-seeking in women does not affect their preference for Dark Triad male faces , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.
[8] Daniel N. Jones,et al. Aggression as a trait: the Dark Tetrad alternative. , 2018, Current opinion in psychology.
[9] D. Sacco,et al. Facing the implications: Dangerous world beliefs differentially predict men and women's aversion to facially communicated psychopathy , 2017 .
[10] D. Sacco,et al. Women’s Dangerous World Beliefs Predict More Accurate Discrimination of Affiliative Facial Cues , 2017 .
[11] D. Perrett,et al. Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity , 2017 .
[12] A. Little,et al. A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior , 2017 .
[13] A. Little,et al. Baseline Probabilities for Two-Alternative Forced Choice Tasks When Judging Stimuli in Evolutionary Psychology: A Methodological Note , 2017 .
[14] P. Jonason,et al. The Exploitive Mating Strategy of the Dark Triad Traits: Tests of Rape-Enabling Attitudes , 2017, Archives of sexual behavior.
[15] Satoru Kiire. Psychopathy rather than Machiavellianism or narcissism facilitates intimate partner violence via fast life strategy , 2017 .
[16] Eli D. Strauss,et al. Aggression and dominance: an interdisciplinary overview , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[17] M. Lyons,et al. Corrigendum to "The effects of resource availability and relationship status on women's preference for facial masculinity in men: An eye-tracking study", [Pers. Individ. Dif. 95 (June 2016) 25–28] , 2016 .
[18] M. Lyons,et al. “I could see, in the depth of his eyes, my own beauty reflected”: Women's assortative preference for narcissistic, but not for Machiavellian or psychopathic male faces , 2016 .
[19] Hannah Ryder,et al. Women's fear of crime and preference for formidable mates: how specific are the underlying psychological mechanisms?☆ , 2016 .
[20] M. Lyons,et al. The effects of resource availability and relationship status on women's preference for facial masculinity in men: An eye-tracking study , 2016 .
[21] B. E. Atkinson,et al. Relationships between age of females and attraction to the Dark Triad personality , 2016 .
[22] M. Lyons,et al. The undesirable Dark Triad? Women dislike Dark Triad male faces across different mating context and socio-ecological conditions , 2016 .
[23] D. Sacco,et al. ACTIVATION OF SELF-PROTECTION THREAT INCREASES WOMEN'S PREFERENCES FOR DOMINANCE IN MALE FACES , 2015 .
[24] Daniel N. Jones,et al. The Dark Triad and dispositional aggression , 2015 .
[25] Vincent Egan,et al. Untangling the concurrent influences of the Dark Triad, personality and mating effort on violence , 2015 .
[26] M. Lyons,et al. Dark traits: Sometimes hot, and sometimes not? Female preferences for Dark Triad faces depend on sociosexuality and contraceptive use , 2015 .
[27] Lei Chang,et al. Good Genes, Good Providers, and Good Fathers: Economic Development Involved in How Women Select a Mate , 2015 .
[28] Scott W. Semenyna,et al. Dominance styles mediate sex differences in Dark Triad traits , 2015 .
[29] M. Lyons,et al. Mirror, mirror, on the wall, who is the most masculine of them all? The Dark Triad, masculinity, and women's mate choice , 2015 .
[30] G. J. Lewis,et al. Perceiving Aggression from Facial Structure: Further Evidence for A Positive Association with Facial Width–To–Height Ratio and Masculinity, but Not for Moderation by Self–Reported Dominance , 2014 .
[31] I. Krams,et al. Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in humans , 2012, Nature Communications.
[32] T. Shackelford,et al. Adaptations to avoid victimization , 2012 .
[33] Brendan P. Zietsch,et al. Experimental evidence that women's mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity , 2011, Biology Letters.
[34] Nicholas S. Holtzman. Facing a psychopath: Detecting the dark triad from emotionally-neutral faces, using prototypes from the Personality Faceaurus , 2011 .
[35] L. Tiokhin,et al. Trade-offs in a dangerous world: women's fear of crime predicts preferences for aggressive and formidable mates , 2011 .
[36] Bernard Tiddeman,et al. Prototyping and Transforming Facial Textures for Perception Research , 2001, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.
[37] C. Coulton,et al. Measuring neighborhood context for young children in an urban area , 1996 .