Not My Protector—Women Have an Aversion to High Dark Triad Faces Irrespective of Childhood or Current Environmental Danger

[1]  Ike E. Onyishi,et al.  Women’s preferences for men’s facial masculinity are strongest under favorable ecological conditions , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[2]  P. Christiansen,et al.  A drunk heart speaks a sober mind: Alcohol does not influence the selection of short-term partners with dark triad traits , 2017, Personality and Individual Differences.

[3]  U. Ehlert,et al.  The influence of income and testosterone on the validity of facial width-to-height ratio as a biomarker for dominance , 2018, PloS one.

[4]  Virgil Zeigler‐Hill,et al.  The fundamental social motives that characterize dark personality traits , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.

[5]  L. DeBruine,et al.  Are dark triad cues really visible in faces? , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.

[6]  E. Bates,et al.  How Dark Personalities Perpetrate Partner and General Aggression in Sweden and the United Kingdom , 2018, Journal of interpersonal violence.

[7]  Gregory L. Carter,et al.  Sensation-seeking in women does not affect their preference for Dark Triad male faces , 2018, Personality and Individual Differences.

[8]  Daniel N. Jones,et al.  Aggression as a trait: the Dark Tetrad alternative. , 2018, Current opinion in psychology.

[9]  D. Sacco,et al.  Facing the implications: Dangerous world beliefs differentially predict men and women's aversion to facially communicated psychopathy , 2017 .

[10]  D. Sacco,et al.  Women’s Dangerous World Beliefs Predict More Accurate Discrimination of Affiliative Facial Cues , 2017 .

[11]  D. Perrett,et al.  Aggressor or protector? Experiences and perceptions of violence predict preferences for masculinity , 2017 .

[12]  A. Little,et al.  A Theoretical Proposal for Examining the Integration of Cooperative and Competitive Mothering Behavior , 2017 .

[13]  A. Little,et al.  Baseline Probabilities for Two-Alternative Forced Choice Tasks When Judging Stimuli in Evolutionary Psychology: A Methodological Note , 2017 .

[14]  P. Jonason,et al.  The Exploitive Mating Strategy of the Dark Triad Traits: Tests of Rape-Enabling Attitudes , 2017, Archives of sexual behavior.

[15]  Satoru Kiire Psychopathy rather than Machiavellianism or narcissism facilitates intimate partner violence via fast life strategy , 2017 .

[16]  Eli D. Strauss,et al.  Aggression and dominance: an interdisciplinary overview , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[17]  M. Lyons,et al.  Corrigendum to "The effects of resource availability and relationship status on women's preference for facial masculinity in men: An eye-tracking study", [Pers. Individ. Dif. 95 (June 2016) 25–28] , 2016 .

[18]  M. Lyons,et al.  “I could see, in the depth of his eyes, my own beauty reflected”: Women's assortative preference for narcissistic, but not for Machiavellian or psychopathic male faces , 2016 .

[19]  Hannah Ryder,et al.  Women's fear of crime and preference for formidable mates: how specific are the underlying psychological mechanisms?☆ , 2016 .

[20]  M. Lyons,et al.  The effects of resource availability and relationship status on women's preference for facial masculinity in men: An eye-tracking study , 2016 .

[21]  B. E. Atkinson,et al.  Relationships between age of females and attraction to the Dark Triad personality , 2016 .

[22]  M. Lyons,et al.  The undesirable Dark Triad? Women dislike Dark Triad male faces across different mating context and socio-ecological conditions , 2016 .

[23]  D. Sacco,et al.  ACTIVATION OF SELF-PROTECTION THREAT INCREASES WOMEN'S PREFERENCES FOR DOMINANCE IN MALE FACES , 2015 .

[24]  Daniel N. Jones,et al.  The Dark Triad and dispositional aggression , 2015 .

[25]  Vincent Egan,et al.  Untangling the concurrent influences of the Dark Triad, personality and mating effort on violence , 2015 .

[26]  M. Lyons,et al.  Dark traits: Sometimes hot, and sometimes not? Female preferences for Dark Triad faces depend on sociosexuality and contraceptive use , 2015 .

[27]  Lei Chang,et al.  Good Genes, Good Providers, and Good Fathers: Economic Development Involved in How Women Select a Mate , 2015 .

[28]  Scott W. Semenyna,et al.  Dominance styles mediate sex differences in Dark Triad traits , 2015 .

[29]  M. Lyons,et al.  Mirror, mirror, on the wall, who is the most masculine of them all? The Dark Triad, masculinity, and women's mate choice , 2015 .

[30]  G. J. Lewis,et al.  Perceiving Aggression from Facial Structure: Further Evidence for A Positive Association with Facial Width–To–Height Ratio and Masculinity, but Not for Moderation by Self–Reported Dominance , 2014 .

[31]  I. Krams,et al.  Evidence for the stress-linked immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in humans , 2012, Nature Communications.

[32]  T. Shackelford,et al.  Adaptations to avoid victimization , 2012 .

[33]  Brendan P. Zietsch,et al.  Experimental evidence that women's mate preferences are directly influenced by cues of pathogen prevalence and resource scarcity , 2011, Biology Letters.

[34]  Nicholas S. Holtzman Facing a psychopath: Detecting the dark triad from emotionally-neutral faces, using prototypes from the Personality Faceaurus , 2011 .

[35]  L. Tiokhin,et al.  Trade-offs in a dangerous world: women's fear of crime predicts preferences for aggressive and formidable mates , 2011 .

[36]  Bernard Tiddeman,et al.  Prototyping and Transforming Facial Textures for Perception Research , 2001, IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications.

[37]  C. Coulton,et al.  Measuring neighborhood context for young children in an urban area , 1996 .