The Cambridge Handbook of Human Affective Neuroscience: Bodily Expressions of Emotion

In the interaction of emotion and physiology, two important conceptualizations can be distinguished: efferent and afferent effects of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) in emotion. Although the afferent and efferent definitions of emotional response are not mutually exclusive, the large majority of research on emotion has been based on the efferent conceptualization. This chapter draws on research findings in support of each conceptualization. It first considers the anatomy and central control of the ANS. The anatomical structure of the ANS and its central control constitute the basis for understanding autonomic effects of emotion. The chapter talks about neural feedback mechanisms, although hormonal, chemical, and physical feedback mechanisms may also operate through interactions with central components of this neural pathway. Various neural feedback mechanisms exist that allow for the transmission of information on homeostatic parameters. The exact nature and role of peripheral physiological responses to emotional feelings remain to be determined.

[1]  P. Haggard,et al.  Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Reveals Two Cortical Pathways for Visual Body Processing , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  T. Poggio,et al.  Cognitive neuroscience: Neural mechanisms for the recognition of biological movements , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[3]  H Poizner,et al.  Visual and "phonetic" coding of movement: evidence from American sign language. , 1981, Science.

[4]  Mark C. Coulson Attributing Emotion to Static Body Postures: Recognition Accuracy, Confusions, and Viewpoint Dependence , 2004 .

[5]  S. Carey,et al.  Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[6]  S. Lea,et al.  Perception of Emotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays Represented in Dance , 1996, Perception.

[7]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy , 2004, NeuroImage.

[8]  G. Mather,et al.  Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Behavioral / Systems / Cognitive Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Investigation of Overlapping Lateral Occipitotemporal Activations Using Multi-Voxel Pattern Analysis , 2006 .

[11]  A. Young,et al.  Emotion Perception from Dynamic and Static Body Expressions in Point-Light and Full-Light Displays , 2004, Perception.

[12]  Ross Buck,et al.  The communication of emotion , 1984 .

[13]  Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,et al.  Separate Face and Body Selectivity on the Fusiform Gyrus , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  B. Hubert,et al.  Brief Report: Recognition of Emotional and Non-emotional Biological Motion in Individuals with Autistic Spectrum Disorders , 2007 .

[15]  G. Johansson Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .

[16]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain Areas Active during Visual Perception of Biological Motion , 2002, Neuron.

[17]  Lars Omlor,et al.  Lateral asymmetry of bodily emotion expression , 2008, Current Biology.

[18]  J. Grèzes,et al.  A failure to grasp the affective meaning of actions in autism spectrum disorder subjects , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[19]  Rick Grush,et al.  The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception , 2004, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[20]  Das Erkennen von Emotionen aus Ausdrucksbewegungen des Gesichts , 1991 .

[21]  P. Downing,et al.  The neural basis of visual body perception , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[22]  Antonino Casile,et al.  Critical features for the recognition of biological motion. , 2005, Journal of vision.

[23]  A. Cowey,et al.  Visual deficits in a patient with `kaleidoscopic disintegration of the visual world' , 2002, European journal of neurology.

[24]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[25]  M. Sawada,et al.  Expression of Emotions in Dance: Relation between Arm Movement Characteristics and Emotion , 2003, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Role of the amygdala in processing visual social stimuli. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[27]  P. McLeod,et al.  Preserved and Impaired Detection of Structure From Motion by a 'Motion-blind" Patient , 1996 .

[28]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala , 1994, Nature.

[29]  Galit Yovel,et al.  It's all in your head: why is the body inversion effect abolished for headless bodies? , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Dissociation between emotion and personality judgments: Convergent evidence from functional neuroimaging , 2005, NeuroImage.

[31]  Nouchine Hadjikhani,et al.  Body expressions of emotion do not trigger fear contagion in autism spectrum disorder. , 2009, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[32]  J. Driver,et al.  Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: windows on causal interactions between human brain regions , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  The role of the extrastriate body area in action perception , 2006, Social neuroscience.

[34]  M. Peelen,et al.  Direct intracranial recording of body-selective responses in human extrastriate visual cortex , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[35]  A. Saygin Superior temporal and premotor brain areas necessary for biological motion perception. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[36]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Neural Integration of Information Specifying Human Structure from Form, Motion, and Depth , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  G. Orban,et al.  Human Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Reveals Separation and Integration of Shape and Motion Cues in Biological Motion Processing , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Armin Bruderlin,et al.  Perceiving affect from arm movement , 2001, Cognition.

[39]  Anthony P. Atkinson,et al.  Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  J A Beintema,et al.  Perception of biological motion without local image motion , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[41]  M. Sereno,et al.  Point-Light Biological Motion Perception Activates Human Premotor Cortex , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  T. Flash,et al.  Neuronal encoding of human kinematic invariants during action observation. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[43]  R. Blake,et al.  Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.

[44]  N. Hadjikhani,et al.  Seeing Fearful Body Expressions Activates the Fusiform Cortex and Amygdala , 2003, Current Biology.

[45]  Swann Pichon,et al.  Perceiving fear in dynamic body expressions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[46]  P. Downing,et al.  The effect of viewpoint on body representation in the extrastriate body area , 2004, Neuroreport.

[47]  J. Montepare,et al.  The identification of emotions from gait information , 1987 .

[48]  S. Runeson,et al.  Visual perception of lifted weight. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[49]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: a 3-D lesion study. , 2002 .

[50]  Catherine L Reed,et al.  Turning configural processing upside down: part and whole body postures. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  B. Gelder,et al.  Why bodies? Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[52]  Patrik Vuilleumier,et al.  Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. , 2007, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[53]  Richard J. Davidson,et al.  Exploring Hindu Indian Emotion Expressions: Evidence for Accurate Recognition by Americans and Indians , 2000, Psychological science.

[54]  N. Birbaumer,et al.  Dissociable cortical processing of recognizable and non-recognizable biological movement: analysing gamma MEG activity. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  R. Desimone,et al.  Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  Bennett I. Bertenthal,et al.  Global Processing of Biological Motions , 1994 .

[57]  H. Przuntek,et al.  Knowing no fear , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[58]  T. Shipley The Effect of Object and Event Orientation on Perception of Biological Motion , 2003, Psychological science.

[59]  R. J Dolan,et al.  Common and distinct neural responses during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions , 2003, NeuroImage.

[60]  C. Darwin The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals , .

[61]  N. Hadjikhani,et al.  Fear fosters flight: a mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[62]  Scott T Grafton,et al.  From 'acting on' to 'acting with': the functional anatomy of object-oriented action schemata. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[63]  B. de Gelder,et al.  Body expressions influence recognition of emotions in the face and voice. , 2007, Emotion.

[64]  Anthony P. Atkinson,et al.  Spared ability to recognise fear from static and moving whole-body cues following bilateral amygdala damage , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[65]  Konstantin Stanislavsky,et al.  An Actor Prepares , 1936 .

[66]  Cosimo Urgesi,et al.  Magnetic Stimulation of Extrastriate Body Area Impairs Visual Processing of Nonfacial Body Parts , 2004, Current Biology.

[67]  R. Adolphs Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms. , 2002, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[68]  J. Movellan,et al.  Human and computer recognition of facial expressions of emotion , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[69]  J. Decety,et al.  From the perception of action to the understanding of intention , 2001, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.

[70]  Kenji Kawano,et al.  Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex , 1999, Nature.

[71]  A. Atkinson,et al.  Neuroscientific Evidence for Simulation and Shared Substrates in Emotion Recognition: Beyond Faces , 2009 .

[72]  R. Buck Social and emotional functions in facial expression and communication: the readout hypothesis , 1994, Biological Psychology.

[73]  Marco Tamietto,et al.  Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[74]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  A unifying view of the basis of social cognition , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[75]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Repetitive TMS over posterior STS disrupts perception of biological motion , 2005, Vision Research.

[76]  P. Ekman,et al.  The Repertoire of Nonverbal Behavior: Categories, Origins, Usage, and Coding , 1969 .

[77]  Cord Westhoff,et al.  Kinematic cues for person identification from biological motion , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.

[78]  R. Adolphs,et al.  A Role for Somatosensory Cortices in the Visual Recognition of Emotion as Revealed by Three-Dimensional Lesion Mapping , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[79]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  TMS disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions , 2010 .

[80]  Daniel Tranel,et al.  Amygdala damage impairs emotion recognition from scenes only when they contain facial expressions , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[81]  Guy Vingerhoets,et al.  What is said or how it is said makes a difference: role of the right fronto‐parietal operculum in emotional prosody as revealed by repetitive TMS , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.

[82]  P. Vuilleumier,et al.  How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[83]  Hannah E. Smithson,et al.  Modulation of the face- and body-selective visual regions by the motion and emotion of point-light face and body stimuli , 2012, NeuroImage.

[84]  A. Johnston,et al.  Comparing Solid-Body with Point-Light Animations , 2003, Perception.

[85]  Lisa Feldman Barrett,et al.  Functional grouping and cortical–subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies , 2008, NeuroImage.

[86]  M. Peelen,et al.  Supramodal Representations of Perceived Emotions in the Human Brain , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[87]  Michael S Beauchamp,et al.  See me, hear me, touch me: multisensory integration in lateral occipital-temporal cortex , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[88]  N. Troje Decomposing biological motion: a framework for analysis and synthesis of human gait patterns. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[89]  Lester M. Hyman,et al.  Which are the stimuli in facial displays of anger and happiness? Configurational bases of emotion recognition. , 1992 .

[90]  R. Dolan,et al.  Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[91]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Cortical Regions for Judgments of Emotions and Personality Traits from Point-light Walkers , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[92]  B. Gelder Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[93]  Claire L. Roether,et al.  Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[94]  M. Candidi,et al.  Representation of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral premotor cortex , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[95]  Aina Puce,et al.  Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[96]  J. N. Bassili Facial motion in the perception of faces and of emotional expression. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[97]  Christine Deruelle,et al.  Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders , 2007 .

[98]  G. Mather,et al.  Low-level visual processing of biological motion , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[99]  M. Shiffrar,et al.  Recognizing people from their movement. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[100]  Michael Andres,et al.  Dissociable roles of the human somatosensory and superior temporal cortices for processing social face signals , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[101]  T. Allison,et al.  Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. II: Response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.

[102]  Steven M. Thurman,et al.  Perceptual and computational analysis of critical features for biological motion. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[103]  K. Doya,et al.  A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[104]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[105]  P. Downing,et al.  Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[106]  R. Laban,et al.  The mastery of movement , 1950 .

[107]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[108]  H. Wallbott Bodily expression of emotion , 1998 .

[109]  William Thomas James,et al.  A Study of the Expression of Bodily Posture , 1932 .

[110]  A. Atkinson,et al.  Evidence for distinct contributions of form and motion information to the recognition of emotions from body gestures , 2007, Cognition.

[111]  Rp Hobson,et al.  Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non‐autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents , 1997 .

[112]  J. Cutting,et al.  Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition , 1978, Perception & psychophysics.

[113]  M. D. Meijer The contribution of general features of body movement to the attribution of emotions , 1989 .

[114]  A. J. Fridlund Evolution and facial action in reflex, social motive, and paralanguage , 1991, Biological Psychology.

[115]  R. Blake,et al.  Brain activity evoked by inverted and imagined biological motion , 2001, Vision Research.

[116]  T. Shallice From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure: Converging Operations: Specific Syndromes and Evidence from Normal Subjects , 1988 .

[117]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Human Body , 2001 .

[118]  Alison J. Wiggett,et al.  Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion , 2006, Neuron.

[119]  J. Stekelenburg,et al.  The neural correlates of perceiving human bodies: an ERP study on the body-inversion effect , 2004, Neuroreport.

[120]  J. Lange,et al.  A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[121]  Boris Suchan,et al.  Losing your Head: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Effects of Body Inversion , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[122]  P. Ekman An argument for basic emotions , 1992 .