Biasing moral decisions by exploiting the dynamics of eye gaze

Significance Where people look generally reflects and reveals their moment-by-moment thought processes. This study introduces an experimental method whereby participants’ eye gaze is monitored and information about their gaze is used to change the timing of their decisions. Answers to difficult moral questions such as “Is murder justifiable?” can be influenced toward random alternatives based on looking patterns alone. We do this without presenting different arguments or response frames, as in other techniques of persuasion. Thus, the process of arriving at a moral decision is not only reflected in a participant’s eye gaze but can also be determined by it. Eye gaze is a window onto cognitive processing in tasks such as spatial memory, linguistic processing, and decision making. We present evidence that information derived from eye gaze can be used to change the course of individuals’ decisions, even when they are reasoning about high-level, moral issues. Previous studies have shown that when an experimenter actively controls what an individual sees the experimenter can affect simple decisions with alternatives of almost equal valence. Here we show that if an experimenter passively knows when individuals move their eyes the experimenter can change complex moral decisions. This causal effect is achieved by simply adjusting the timing of the decisions. We monitored participants’ eye movements during a two-alternative forced-choice task with moral questions. One option was randomly predetermined as a target. At the moment participants had fixated the target option for a set amount of time we terminated their deliberation and prompted them to choose between the two alternatives. Although participants were unaware of this gaze-contingent manipulation, their choices were systematically biased toward the target option. We conclude that even abstract moral cognition is partly constituted by interactions with the immediate environment and is likely supported by gaze-dependent decision processes. By tracking the interplay between individuals, their sensorimotor systems, and the environment, we can influence the outcome of a decision without directly manipulating the content of the information available to them.

[1]  Shaun Nichols,et al.  Moral dilemmas and moral rules , 2006, Cognition.

[2]  A. Rangel,et al.  Biasing simple choices by manipulating relative visual attention , 2008, Judgment and Decision Making.

[3]  L. Petrinovich,et al.  Influence of Wording and Framing Effects on Moral Intuitions , 1996 .

[4]  Petter Johansson,et al.  Failure to Detect Mismatches Between Intention and Outcome in a Simple Decision Task , 2005, Science.

[5]  M. Shadlen,et al.  Exploring the Neurophysiology of Decisions , 1998, Neuron.

[6]  F. Cushman Action, Outcome, and Value , 2013, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[7]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Conversation, Gaze Coordination, and Beliefs About Visual Context , 2009, Cogn. Sci..

[8]  M. S. Mayzner,et al.  Cognition And Reality , 1976 .

[9]  Julie C. Sedivy,et al.  Subject Terms: Linguistics Language Eyes & eyesight Cognition & reasoning , 1995 .

[10]  A. Rangel,et al.  Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  Daniel M. Bartels,et al.  Moral Judgment and Decision Making , 2014 .

[12]  James Dungan,et al.  Where in the brain is morality? Everywhere and maybe nowhere , 2012, Social neuroscience.

[13]  R. Bogacz Optimal decision-making theories: linking neurobiology with behaviour , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[14]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Mapping the moral domain. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Multimodal events and moving locations: eye movements of adults and 6-month-olds reveal dynamic spatial indexing. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[16]  Cleotilde González,et al.  How choice ecology influences search in decisions from experience , 2012, Cognition.

[17]  Flavio T. P. Oliveira,et al.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation of posterior parietal cortex affects decisions of hand choice , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  William A. Cunningham,et al.  Evaluation is a dynamic process: Moving beyond dual system models. , 2012 .

[19]  J. Gold,et al.  Representation of a perceptual decision in developing oculomotor commands , 2000, Nature.

[20]  Nick Chater,et al.  Salience driven value integration explains decision biases and preference reversal , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[21]  Petter Johansson,et al.  How the Polls Can Be Both Spot On and Dead Wrong: Using Choice Blindness to Shift Political Attitudes and Voter Intentions , 2013, PloS one.

[22]  S. Shimojo,et al.  Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  Ian Krajbich,et al.  Visual fixations and the computation and comparison of value in simple choice , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  D. Robinson,et al.  Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements , 1996, Nature.

[25]  Brian A. Nosek,et al.  Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[26]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Multialternative decision field theory: a dynamic connectionist model of decision making. , 2001, Psychological review.

[27]  J. Haidt The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. , 2001, Psychological review.

[28]  Xiao-Jing Wang,et al.  A Recurrent Network Mechanism of Time Integration in Perceptual Decisions , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[29]  T. Wheatley,et al.  Hypnotic Disgust Makes Moral Judgments More Severe , 2005, Psychological science.

[30]  D. Medin,et al.  Moral kinematics: The role of physical factors in moral judgments , 2012, Memory & Cognition.

[31]  Gideon Keren,et al.  The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgement and decision making , 2015 .

[32]  Gordon D. A. Brown,et al.  Decision by sampling , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.

[33]  Joshua D. Greene,et al.  Moral Judgments Recruit Domain-General Valuation Mechanisms to Integrate Representations of Probability and Magnitude , 2010, Neuron.

[34]  J. Haidt The New Synthesis in Moral Psychology , 2007, Science.

[35]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements , 2007, Nature.

[36]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  Action Dynamics Reveal Parallel Competition in Decision Making , 2008, Psychological science.

[37]  J. Kalaska,et al.  Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action , 2005, Neuron.

[38]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  Representation, space and Hollywood Squares: looking at things that aren't there anymore , 2000, Cognition.

[39]  M. Crockett Models of morality , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[40]  W. E. Baker,et al.  When Can Affective Conditioning and Mere Exposure Directly Influence Brand Choice , 1999 .

[41]  J. Summers,et al.  Decision making and action implementation: evidence for an early visually triggered motor activation specific to potential actions. , 2013, Psychophysiology.

[42]  Marius Usher,et al.  Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[43]  George Wu,et al.  The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making: Keren/The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making , 2015 .

[44]  Laura E. Thomas,et al.  Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[45]  P. Cisek Making decisions through a distributed consensus , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment , 2001, Science.

[47]  Joseph G. Johnson,et al.  The response dynamics of preferential choice , 2013, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  U. Neisser Cognition and reality: principles and implications , 1976 .

[49]  Michael J. Spivey,et al.  The Continuity Of Mind , 2008 .

[50]  L. Skitka The Psychology of Moral Conviction , 2010 .

[51]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[52]  U. Neisser Cognitive Psychology. (Book Reviews: Cognition and Reality. Principles and Implications of Cognitive Psychology) , 1976 .

[53]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[54]  Rick Dale,et al.  The Self-Organization of Explicit Attitudes , 2009, Psychological science.

[55]  C. Koch,et al.  Relative visual saliency differences induce sizable bias in consumer choice , 2012 .

[56]  E. Page‐Gould,et al.  Listen to your heart: when false somatic feedback shapes moral behavior. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[57]  J. Haidt,et al.  Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? , 1993, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[58]  Fiery Cushman,et al.  Patterns of Moral Judgment Derive From Nonmoral Psychological Representations , 2011, Cogn. Sci..

[59]  Daniel C. Richardson,et al.  When facts go down the rabbit hole: Contrasting features and objecthood as indexes to memory , 2008, Cognition.