Asymmetrical adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles.

We report the extent to which effective asymmetrical saccadic adaptation was achieved by a myopic subject, who was exposed to "long-term" adaptation as he wore anisometropic corrective spectacles for about 40 years and also the extent of "short-term" adaptation in this subject and two other subjects, who initially made conjugate saccades, when they wore newly fitted anisometropic spectacles for about 8 hr. Two-dimensional binocular eye positions were measured with an accurate and precise revolving magnetic field-sensor coil technique. We found that long-term adaptation of vertical saccades was virtually perfect (almost 100% of the asymmetry introduced by the spectacles was corrected). Long-term adaptation of horizontal saccades was less complete and increased with target separation from about 40% for saccadic amplitudes of 5 degrees to about 75% for amplitudes of 60 degrees. Short-term adaptation of vertical saccades was virtually complete (100%) in one newly fitted subject and only partially complete (40%) in the other two subjects. The persistence of the adaptive asymmetry of saccades during monocular viewing showed that adaptation derived from plasticity in the programming of saccades and not from modification of vergence responses. Without the anisometropic spectacles, 30 min of self-paced, one per second changes in binocular fixation between two targets, which required a version change of 45 degrees in combination with a vergence change of 11 degrees, did not induce any asymmetrical adaptation. This result shows that a specific repeated association of version and vergence eye movements was not sufficient to induce asymmetrical adaptation, leading us to suggest that the transient fixation disparities at saccade-offset might be the necessary stimulus for the asymmetrical saccadic adaptation we observed.

[1]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Differences in accuracy of human saccades between stationary and jumping targets , 1989, Vision Research.

[2]  H Collewijn,et al.  Ocular vergence under natural conditions. II. Gaze shifts between real targets differing in distance and direction , 1989, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. B. Biological Sciences.

[3]  C. Prablanc,et al.  Kinematics of centrifugal and centripetal saccadic eye movements in man , 1988, Vision Research.

[4]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human vertical saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[5]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Binocular co‐ordination of human horizontal saccadic eye movements. , 1988, The Journal of physiology.

[6]  T. Vilis,et al.  Monocular adaptation of the saccadic system and vestibulo-ocular reflex. , 1988, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[7]  D. Sparks,et al.  Eye movements induced by pontine stimulation: interaction with visually triggered saccades. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  D. Robinson,et al.  Saccadic undershoot is not inevitable: Saccades can be accurate , 1986, Vision Research.

[9]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Eye movements and stereopsis during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms , 1985, Vision Research.

[10]  T Vilis,et al.  Adaptation of saccadic and vestibulo-ocular systems after extraocular muscle tenectomy. , 1985, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[11]  R M Steinman,et al.  Vision in the presence of known natural retinal image motion. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[12]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—IV. Anticipatory smooth eye movements depend on prior target motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[13]  J. T. Enright Changes in vergence mediated by saccades. , 1984, The Journal of physiology.

[14]  R M Steinman,et al.  Compensatory eye movements during active and passive head movements: fast adaptation to changes in visual magnification. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  D B Henson,et al.  Oculomotor adaptation to induced heterophoria and anisometropia. , 1982, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[16]  Eileen Kowler,et al.  The effect of expectations on slow oculomotor control—III. Guessing unpredictable target displacements , 1981, Vision Research.

[17]  R M Steinman,et al.  NATURAL RETINAL IMAGE MOTION: ORIGIN AND CHANGE * , 1981, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[18]  W. B. Templeton,et al.  Saccadic plasticity: parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  L. Optican,et al.  Cerebellar-dependent adaptive control of primate saccadic system. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[20]  D B Henson,et al.  Adaptation to Prism‐Induced Heterophoria , 1980, American journal of optometry and physiological optics.

[21]  D. Whitteridge Movements of the eyes R. H. S. Carpenter, Pion Ltd, London (1977), 420 pp., $27.00 , 1979, Neuroscience.

[22]  Hiroshi Ono,et al.  Changing fixation in the transverse plane at eye level and Hering's law of equal innervation , 1978, Vision Research.

[23]  L. Dell’Osso,et al.  Saccadic system plasticity in humans , 1978, Annals of neurology.

[24]  V. Henn,et al.  Vertical eye movement related unit activity in the rostral mesencephalic reticular formation of the alert monkey , 1977, Brain Research.

[25]  H. Collewijn Eye‐ and head movements in freely moving rabbits. , 1977, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  W. Becker Do correction saccades depend exclusively on retinal feedback? A note on the possible role of non-retinal feedback , 1976, Vision Research.

[27]  P. E. Hallett,et al.  Saccadic eye movements to flashed targets , 1976, Vision Research.

[28]  G. Kommerell,et al.  Adaptive programming of phasic and tonic components in saccadic eye movements. Investigations of patients with abducens palsy. , 1976, Investigative ophthalmology.

[29]  B. Cohen,et al.  Coding of information about rapid eye movements in the pontine reticular formation of alert monkeys , 1976, Brain Research.

[30]  H. Collewijn,et al.  Precise recording of human eye movements , 1975, Vision Research.

[31]  D. C. Allen VERTICAL PRISM ADAPTATION IN ANISOMETROPES* , 1974, American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics.

[32]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Activity of brain stem neurons during eye movements of alert monkeys. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  B. Julesz,et al.  Extension of Panum's fusional area in binocularly stabilized vision. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[34]  D. Robinson,et al.  A METHOD OF MEASURING EYE MOVEMENT USING A SCLERAL SEARCH COIL IN A MAGNETIC FIELD. , 1963, IEEE transactions on bio-medical engineering.

[35]  Ellerbrock Vj Further study of effects induced by anisometropic corrections. , 1948 .

[36]  G. Fry,et al.  EFFECTS INDUCED BY ANISOMETROPIC CORRECTIONS , 1942 .

[37]  H. Deubel ADAPTIVITY OF GAIN AND DIRECTION IN OBLIQUE SACCADES1 , 1987 .

[38]  H Deubel,et al.  Adaptive gain control of saccadic eye movements. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[39]  L M Optican,et al.  Adaptive properties of the saccadic system. , 1985, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[40]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control : facts and theories , 1985 .

[41]  L. Stark,et al.  The Theory of Binocular Vision , 1977, Springer US.

[42]  J. Miller Aging changes in extraocular muscle , 1975 .

[43]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .

[44]  A. L. Yarbus,et al.  Eye Movements and Vision , 1967, Springer US.

[45]  Hugh Davson,et al.  Visual optics and the optical space sense , 1962 .

[46]  V. Ellerbrock Further study of effects induced by anisometropic corrections. , 1948, American journal of optometry and archives of American Academy of Optometry.

[47]  Harry George Critchley,et al.  Ametropia and its correction , 1891 .