Asymmetric response time functions during left-/right-facing discriminations of rotated objects: The short and the long of it
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Hamm,et al. Individual differences in the mixture ratio of rotation and nonrotation trials during rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations , 2012, Memory & cognition.
[2] Stephen M. Kosslyn,et al. Mental rotation is not easily cognitively penetrable , 2010 .
[3] J. Hamm,et al. A model of rotated mirror/normal letter discriminations , 2010, Memory & cognition.
[4] M. Hausmann,et al. Sex-specific response strategies in mental rotation , 2009 .
[5] J. Hamm,et al. Stimulus-response compatibilities during top-bottom discriminations. , 2008, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[6] M. Heil,et al. Sex Differences in Mental Rotation with Polygons of Different Complexity: Do Men Utilize Holistic Processes whereas Women Prefer Piecemeal Ones? , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[7] Petra Jansen-Osmann,et al. Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation , 2007, Brain and Cognition.
[8] Daniel Voyer,et al. Sex differences in mental rotation: Top–down versus bottom–up processing , 2006, NeuroImage.
[9] David R. Anderson,et al. Model selection and multimodel inference : a practical information-theoretic approach , 2003 .
[10] M. Bar. A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Manila Vannucci,et al. Drawing and identifying objects in relation to semantic category and handedness , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[12] P. Mcmullen,et al. Effects of orientation on the identification of rotated objects depend on the level of identity. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[13] Susan D. Voyer,et al. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.
[14] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A Solution to the Effect of Sample Size on Outlier Elimination , 1994 .
[15] P. Jolicoeur,et al. Top-bottom and front-behind decisions on rotated objects. , 1993, Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale.
[16] P. Jolicoeur. Mental rotation and the identification of disoriented objects. , 1988, Canadian journal of psychology.
[17] M. Corballis. Recognition of disoriented shapes. , 1988, Psychological review.
[18] M. Linn,et al. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. , 1985, Child development.
[19] P. Jolicoeur. The time to name disoriented natural objects , 1985, Memory & cognition.
[20] J. G. Snodgrass,et al. A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.
[21] S. Vandenberg,et al. Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.
[22] R. Shepard,et al. CHRONOMETRIC STUDIES OF THE ROTATION OF MENTAL IMAGES , 1973 .
[23] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[24] R. Shepard,et al. Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects , 1971, Science.
[25] R. C. Oldfield. THE ASSESSMENT AND ANALYSIS OF HANDEDNESS , 1971 .