Distinct Regions of the Periaqueductal Gray Are Involved in the Acquisition and Expression of Defensive Responses

In fear conditioning, a rat is placed in a distinct environment and delivered footshock. The response to the footshock itself is called an activity burst and includes running, jumping, and vocalization. The fear conditioned to the distinct environment by the footshock elicits complete immobility termed freezing. Lesions of the ventral periaqueductal gray (vPAG) strongly attenuate freezing. However, lesions of the dorsolateral periaqueductal gray (dlPAG) increase the amount of freezing seen to conditional fear cues acquired under conditions in which intact rats do not demonstrate much fear conditioning. To examine the necessity of these regions in the acquisition and expression of fear, we performed five experiments that examined the effects of electrolytic lesions of the dlPAG and the vPAG in learned and unlearned fear. In experiment 1, lesions of the vPAG strongly attenuated, whereas lesions of the dlPAG enhanced, unconditional freezing to a cat. In experiment 2, lesions of the dlPAG made before but not after training enhanced the amount of freezing shown to conditional fear cues acquired via immediate footshock delivery. In experiment 3, vPAG lesions made either before or after training with footshock decreased the level of freezing to conditional fear cues. Neither dlPAG lesions nor vPAG lesions affected footshock sensitivity (experiment 4) or consumption on a conditioned taste aversion test that does not elicit antipredator responses (experiment 5). On the basis of these results, it is proposed that activation of the dlPAG produces inhibition of the vPAG and forebrain structures involved with defense. In contrast, the vPAG seems to be necessary for postencounter freezing defensive behavior.

[1]  Shigenori Watanabe,et al.  Cardiovascular changes induced by chemical stimulation of the amygdala in rats , 1991, Brain Research Bulletin.

[2]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Conditional and unconditional components of post-shock freezing , 1980, The Pavlovian journal of biological science.

[3]  M. Fanselow The Midbrain Periaqueductal Gray as a Coordinator of Action in Response to Fear and Anxiety , 1991 .

[4]  R. Boylan,et al.  Tonic immobility in Japanese quail can reduce the probability of sustained attack by cats , 1981 .

[5]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Ventral and dorsolateral regions of the midbrain periaqueductal gray (PAG) control different stages of defensive behavior: Dorsolateral PAG lesions enhance the defensive freezing produced by massed and immediate shock , 1995 .

[6]  Mark D. Underwood,et al.  Autonomic and somatomotor effects of amygdala central N. stimulation in awake rabbits , 1983, Physiology & Behavior.

[7]  M. Fanselow Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Michael Davis,et al.  Conditioned fear and startle magnitude: effects of different footshock or backshock intensities used in training. , 1978, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  M. Fanselow,et al.  The Amygdala and Fear Conditioning: Has the Nut Been Cracked? , 1996, Neuron.

[10]  M. Behbehani,et al.  An electrophysiological characterization of the projection from the central nucleus of the amygdala to the periaqueductal gray of the rat: the role of opioid receptors , 1995, Brain Research.

[11]  R. Bolles,et al.  The effect of predictive cues on freezing in rats , 1976 .

[12]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Different projections of the central amygdaloid nucleus mediate autonomic and behavioral correlates of conditioned fear , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[13]  Michael S. Fanselow,et al.  A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: Predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. , 1988 .

[14]  M. Behbehani Functional characteristics of the midbrain periaqueductal gray , 1995, Progress in Neurobiology.

[15]  FJ Helmstetter,et al.  Lesions of the periaqueductal gray and rostral ventromedial medulla disrupt antinociceptive but not cardiovascular aversive conditional responses , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[16]  P. Bellgowan,et al.  Effects of muscimol applied to the basolateral amygdala on acquisition and expression of contextual fear conditioning in rats. , 1994, Behavioral neuroscience.

[17]  W. Willis,et al.  The efferent projections of the periaqueductal gray in the rat: A Phaseolus vulgaris‐leucoagglutinin study. I. Ascending projections , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[18]  M. Fanselow Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: Implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions ☆ , 1986 .

[19]  R. N. Leaton,et al.  Influence of long-term sensitization on long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response in rats: central gray lesions, preexposure, and extinction. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[20]  Joseph E LeDoux Emotional networks and motor control: a fearful view. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[21]  A. Siegel,et al.  An enkephalinergic mechanism involved in amygdaloid suppression of affective defence behavior elicited from the midbrain periaqueductal gray in the cat , 1991, Brain Research.

[22]  T. Lovick Interactions Between Descending Pathways from the Dorsal and Ventrolateral Periaqueductal Gray Matter in the Rat , 1991 .

[23]  M Ennis,et al.  Connections between the central nucleus of the amygdala and the midbrain periaqueductal gray: Topography and reciprocity , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[24]  D. Blanchard,et al.  Defensive reactions in the albino rat , 1971 .

[25]  L. E. Eberhardt,et al.  Death feigning by ducks in response to predation by red foxes (Vulpes fulva) , 1975 .

[26]  M. Davis,et al.  Involvement of the dorsal periaqueductal gray in the loss of fear-potentiated startle accompanying high footshock training. , 1997, Behavioral neuroscience.

[27]  M. T. Shipley,et al.  Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray: modules for emotional expression? , 1994, Trends in Neurosciences.

[28]  W. Willis,et al.  The efferent projections of the periaqueductal gray in the rat: A Phaseolus vulgaris‐leucoagglutinin study. II. Descending projections , 1995, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[29]  R. N. Leaton,et al.  Potentiated Startle: Its Relation to Freezing and Shock Intensity in Rats , 1985 .

[30]  Mechanisms responsible for reduced contextual conditioning with massed unsignaled unconditional stimuli. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[31]  P. Bellgowan,et al.  Neural systems for the expression of hypoalgesia during nonassociative fear. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Associative regulation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: unconditional stimulus intensity, incentive shifts, and latent inhibition. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[33]  D C Blanchard,et al.  Crouching as an index of fear. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[34]  M. Fanselow,et al.  Contextual conditioning with massed versus distributed unconditional stimuli in the absence of explicit conditional stimuli. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.