The Enigma of Absolute Pitch

Figure 1. The pitch class circle. The notes of the traditional Western musical scale are produced by dividing the octave into 12 semitone steps. A semitone is the pitch relation formed by two adjacent notes on a keyboard, and corresponds to a frequency ratio of approximately 18:17. Each of the twelve notes within the octave is assigned a name: C, C#, D; D#, E, F, F#, G, G#, A, A#, and B. The entire scale is generated by repeating this series of note names (or ‘pitch classes’) across octaves. When people with absolute pitch identify notes by name, they are identifying the positions of the tones along the pitch class circle. Introduction In the summer of 1763, the Mozart family embarked on the famous tour of Europe that established the young composer’s reputation as a musical prodigy. Just before they left, an anonymous letter appeared in the Augsburgischer Intelligenz-Zettel describing seven year old Wolfgang’s extraordinary abilities. The letter included the following:

[1]  M. Dolson The Pitch of Speech as a Function of Linguistic Community , 1994 .

[2]  Siripong Potisuk,et al.  Lexical tones in Thai after unilateral brain damage , 1992, Brain and Language.

[3]  J. Saffran,et al.  Absolute pitch in infant auditory learning: evidence for developmental reorganization. , 2001, Developmental psychology.

[4]  D. Deutsch Paradoxes of musical pitch. , 1992, Scientific American.

[5]  S. L. Campbell,et al.  Frequency and frequency-ratio resolution by possessors of absolute and relative pitch: examples of categorical perception. , 1994, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[6]  R. Shepard Circularity in Judgments of Relative Pitch , 1964 .

[7]  A. H. Takeuchi,et al.  Absolute pitch. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  P. Brady,et al.  Fixed-scale mechanism of absolute pitch. , 1970, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[9]  B. MacWhinney,et al.  Language Development , 2015 .

[10]  I. Peretz,et al.  The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music , 2003 .

[11]  D. Levitin Absolute memory for musical pitch: Evidence from the production of learned melodies , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  Otto Erich Deutsch,et al.  Mozart, a documentary biography , 1965 .

[13]  E. Terhardt,et al.  Aural key identification and its relationship to absolute pitch. , 1983 .

[14]  L. Petitto,et al.  Biological Foundations of Language , 1967, Neurology.

[15]  N. Geschwind,et al.  Color-naming defects in association with alexia. , 1966, Transactions of the American Neurological Association.

[16]  Robert O. Gjerdingen,et al.  The psychology of music , 2002 .

[17]  S. Curtiss Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day "Wild Child" , 1977 .

[18]  Diana Deutsch,et al.  The Tritone Paradox: Correlate with the Listener's Vocal Range for Speech , 1990 .

[19]  G. Grisetti,et al.  Further Reading , 1984, IEEE Spectrum.

[20]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  T. G. Bidder,et al.  Perfect pitch. , 1988, American journal of medical genetics.

[22]  D. Deutsch,et al.  Absolute Pitch, Speech, and Tone Language: Some Experiments and a Proposed Framework , 2004 .

[23]  N. Freimer,et al.  Absolute pitch: an approach for identification of genetic and nongenetic components. , 1998, American journal of human genetics.

[24]  Andrzej Rakowski,et al.  Absolute pitch: Common traits in music and language , 2007 .

[25]  G. Schlaug,et al.  In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians , 1995, Science.

[26]  P. Gregersen,et al.  Early childhood music education and predisposition to absolute pitch: teasing apart genes and environment. , 2001, American journal of medical genetics.

[27]  D. Deutsch The Tritone Paradox: An Influence of Language on Music Perception , 1991 .

[28]  Gregory R. Lockhead,et al.  Practically perfect pitch , 1981 .

[29]  P. Gorelick,et al.  The aprosodias: further functional-anatomical evidence for the organisation of affective language in the right hemisphere. , 1987, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[30]  D. Deutsch,et al.  Absolute pitch among American and Chinese conservatory students: prevalence differences, and evidence for a speech-related critical period. , 2006, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[31]  Ken'ichi Miyazaki,et al.  Perception of Musical Intervals by Absolute Pitch Possessors , 1992 .

[32]  E. Lenneberg Biological Foundations of Language , 1967 .

[33]  D. Deutsch,et al.  Speech Patterns Heard Early in Life Influence Later Perception of the Tritone Paradox , 2004 .

[34]  Jerold A. Edmondson,et al.  The effect of right-brain damage on acoustical measures of affective prosody in Taiwanese patients , 1987 .

[35]  A. Halpern,et al.  Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs , 1989, Memory & cognition.

[36]  J. S. Johnson,et al.  Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language , 1989, Cognitive Psychology.