Mental Imagery of Visual Motion Modifies the Perception of Roll-Vection Stimulation
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Held,et al. Moving Visual Scenes Influence the Apparent Direction of Gravity , 1972, Science.
[2] L R Young,et al. Interaction of optokinetic and vestibular stimuli in motion perception. , 1973, Acta oto-laryngologica.
[3] L. Young,et al. Vestibular nucleus units in alert monkeys are also influenced by moving visual fields. , 1974, Brain research.
[4] Johannes Dichgans,et al. Characteristics of moving visual scenes influencing spatial orientation , 1975, Vision Research.
[5] L. Cooper. Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation , 1976 .
[6] A. Berthoz,et al. Linear Acceleration Modifies the Perceived Velocity of a Moving Visual Scene , 1977, Perception.
[7] J. Lackner,et al. Changes in apparent body orientation and sensory localization induced by vibration of postural muscles: vibratory myesthetic illusions. , 1979, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.
[8] R. Shepard,et al. Mental Images and Their Transformations , 1982 .
[9] R. McLaren,et al. Interaction between perceived and imagined rotation. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] S. Kosslyn,et al. Individual differences in mental imagery ability: A computational analysis , 1984, Cognition.
[11] M. Braunstein,et al. Induced self-motion in central vision. , 1985, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[12] George J. Andersen,et al. Induced self-motion in central vision. , 1985 .
[13] A Reeves,et al. Visual Imagery Selectively Reduces Vernier Acuity , 1987, Perception.
[14] T. Heckmann,et al. Circular Vection as a Function of the Relative Sizes, Distances, and Positions of Two Competing Visual Displays , 1989, Perception.
[15] L. Yardley. Contribution of somatosensory information to perception of the visual vertical with body tilt and rotating visual field , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[16] O J Grüsser,et al. Localization and responses of neurones in the parieto‐insular vestibular cortex of awake monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). , 1990, The Journal of physiology.
[17] Horst Mittelstaedt,et al. The role of the otoliths in the perception of the orientation of self and world to the vertical , 1991 .
[18] J. Dannemiller,et al. Role of image acceleration in judging landing location of free-falling projectiles. , 1993 .
[19] B. C. Motter. Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[20] I. Howard,et al. Vection: The Contributions of Absolute and Relative Visual Motion , 1994, Perception.
[21] P. Baudonniere,et al. Cognitive Effects on Visually Induced Body Motion in Children , 1995, Perception.
[22] S. Kosslyn,et al. Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex , 1995, Nature.
[23] R. Blake,et al. Neural Adaptation of Imaginary Visual Motion , 1995, Cognitive Psychology.
[24] L. Parsons,et al. Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET , 1995, Nature.
[25] S. Shimojo,et al. Assimilation-type and Contrast-type Bias of Motion Induced by the Surround in a Random-dot Display: Evidence for Center-Surround Antagonism , 1996, Vision Research.
[26] Fred Mast,et al. Perceived body position and the visual horizontal , 1996, Brain Research Bulletin.
[27] R S Kennedy,et al. Psychophysical scaling of circular vection (CV) produced by optokinetic (OKN) motion: individual differences and effects of practice. , 1996, Journal of vestibular research : equilibrium & orientation.
[28] A. Treisman,et al. Voluntary Attention Modulates fMRI Activity in Human MT–MST , 1997, Neuron.
[29] F. Rösler,et al. A rotation aftereffect changes both the speed and the preferred direction of mental rotation. , 1997 .
[30] S. Kosslyn,et al. Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. , 1998, Psychophysiology.
[31] T. Brandt,et al. Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[32] I. Curthoys,et al. Visually perceived vertical and visually perceived horizontal are not orthogonal , 1998, Vision Research.
[33] M. Guerraz,et al. The role of head-centric spatial reference with a static and kinetic visual disturbance , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[34] A. Berthoz,et al. Functional MRI of galvanic vestibular stimulation. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[35] A. Berthoz,et al. Multisensory integration in spatial orientation , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[36] D M Merfeld,et al. Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration , 1999, Nature.
[37] S M Kosslyn,et al. Visual mental imagery interferes with allocentric orientation judgements. , 1999, Neuroreport.
[38] S. Kosslyn,et al. The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. , 1999, Science.
[39] S. Shimojo,et al. Critical Role of Foreground Stimuli in Perceiving Visually Induced Self-Motion (Vection) , 1999, Perception.
[40] I. Howard,et al. Effect of Field Size, Head Motion, and Rotational Velocity on Roll Vection and Illusory Self-Tilt in a Tumbling Room , 1999, Perception.
[41] J. V. Van Gisbergen,et al. Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.
[42] T Mergner,et al. Visual contributions to human self-motion perception during horizontal body rotation. , 2000, Archives italiennes de biologie.