Fish can encode order in their spatial map.

Animals must often orient through areas that are larger than their perceptual range. The blind Mexican cave fish, Astyanax fasciatus, depends on detecting self-induced near-field wave perturbations by objects via the use of its lateral line organ. Its perceptual range (less than or equal to 0.05 m) is greatly exceeded by its ecological ranging requirements (ca. 30 m). Although known to possess a spatial map of its environment, it is not known how this fish links places (or the area over which the perceptual range extends) together. Using the blind cave fish's propensity to accelerate when faced with objects or environments that are recognizably different, I used a behavioural assay to test whether fishes can learn and remember the order of a landmark sequence. I show, to my knowledge for the first time, that blind Mexican cave fish can encode order in their spatial map. The ability to represent the order in which a series of places are spatially linked is a powerful tool for animals that must orient beyond the limit of their perceptual range. The resulting spatial map would be analogous to a jigsaw puzzle, where each piece represents a place whose size is constrained by the animal's perceptual range.

[1]  G. Miller,et al.  The Role of Learning. , 1951 .

[2]  H. L. Roitblat,et al.  Performance ofBetta splendens in a radial arm maze , 1982 .

[3]  E. Hassan Hydrodynamic Imaging of the Surroundings by the Lateral Line of the Blind Cave Fish Anoptichthys jordani , 1989 .

[4]  D. Compton Serial learning: A review of the behavioral and physiological research with the rat , 1991, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[5]  K. Swartz,et al.  Serial learning by rhesus monkeys: I. Acquisition and retention of multiple four-item lists. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[6]  Herbert S. Terrace,et al.  The Phylogeny and Ontogeny of Serial Memory: List Learning by Pigeons and Monkeys , 1993 .

[7]  Juan P. Vargas,et al.  Performance of goldfish trained in allocentric and egocentric maze procedures suggests the presence of a cognitive mapping system in fishes , 1994 .

[8]  Zhang,et al.  Honeybee navigation en route to the goal: visual flight control and odometry , 1996, The Journal of experimental biology.

[9]  Zhang,et al.  Visually mediated odometry in honeybees , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[10]  S. Healy Spatial representation in animals. , 1998 .

[11]  D. Biro,et al.  NUMERICAL ORDERING IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) : PLANNING, EXECUTING, AND MONITORING , 1999 .

[12]  J. Krause,et al.  Learning in fishes: From three-second memory to culture , 2003 .

[13]  F. Rodríguez,et al.  Spatial cognition and its neural basis in teleost fishes , 2003 .

[14]  J. Montgomery,et al.  Sensory integration in the hydrodynamic world of rainbow trout , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[15]  V. Braithwaite,et al.  The role of learning in fish orientation , 2003 .

[16]  K. Warburton Learning of foraging skills by fish , 2003 .

[17]  S. N. Fry,et al.  Sequence learning by honeybees , 1993, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[18]  T. Teyke,et al.  Flow field, swimming velocity and boundary layer: parameters which affect the stimulus for the lateral line organ in blind fish , 1988, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[19]  Theresa Burt de Perera,et al.  Spatial parameters encoded in the spatial map of the blind Mexican cave fish, Astyanax fasciatus , 2004, Animal Behaviour.

[20]  Ernst S. Reese,et al.  Orientation behavior of butterflyfishes (family Chaetodontidae) on coral reefs: spatial learning of route specific landmarks and cognitive maps , 1989, Environmental Biology of Fishes.

[21]  T. Teyke,et al.  Learning and remembering the environment in the blind cave fishAnoptichthys jordani , 1989, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[22]  El-S. Hassan,et al.  Mathematical analysis of the stimulus for the lateral line organ , 1985, Biological Cybernetics.