Having a goal to stop action is associated with advance control of specific motor representations

An important aspect of cognitive control consists in the ability to stop oneself from making inappropriate responses. In an earlier study we demonstrated that there are different mechanisms for stopping: global and selective [Aron, A. R., Verbruggen, F. (2008). Stop the presses: Dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping. Psychological Science, 19(11) 1146-1153]. We argued that participants are more likely to use a global mechanism when speed is of the essence, whereas they are more likely to use a selective mechanism when they have foreknowledge of which response tendency they may need to stop. Here we further investigate the relationship between foreknowledge and selective stopping. In Experiment 1 we adapted the earlier design to show that individual differences in recall accuracy for the stopping goal correlate with the selectivity of the stopping. This confirms that encoding and using a foreknowledge memory cue is a key enabler for a selective stopping mechanism. In Experiment 2, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), to test the hypothesis that foreknowledge "sets up" a control set whereby control is applied onto the response representation that may need to be stopped in the future. We applied TMS to the left motor cortex and measured motor evoked potentials (MEPs) from the right hand while participants performed a similar behavioral paradigm as Experiment 1. In the foreknowledge period, MEPs were significantly reduced for trials where the right hand was the one that might need to be stopped relative to when it was not. This shows that having a goal of what response may need to be stopped in the future consists in applying advance control onto a specific motor representation.

[1]  P. Haggard,et al.  Effects of motor preparation and spatial attention on corticospinal excitability in a delayed-response paradigm , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  P. Rossini,et al.  Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots: basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application. Report of an IFCN committee. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  G. Logan,et al.  The Development of Selective Inhibitory Control Across the Life Span , 2002, Developmental neuropsychology.

[4]  T. Mima,et al.  Suppression of human cortico-motoneuronal excitability during the Stop-signal task , 2009, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[5]  Mark Hallett,et al.  Effect of volitional inhibition on cortical inhibitory mechanisms. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  鯨井 隆 Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex , 1994 .

[7]  Trevor W. Robbins Functioning of frontostriatal anatomical "loops" in mechanisms of cognitive control , 2000 .

[8]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  How to Stop and Change a Response: the Role of Goal Activation in Multitasking , 2022 .

[9]  G. Logan,et al.  Selective Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Off and On Stimulant Medication , 2003, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[10]  M Hallett,et al.  Human corticospinal excitability evaluated with transcranial magnetic stimulation during different reaction time paradigms. , 2000, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[11]  P. Brown,et al.  Event-related beta desynchronization in human subthalamic nucleus correlates with motor performance. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[12]  J. Driver,et al.  Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII , 2000 .

[13]  P. Pollux,et al.  Advance preparation of set-switches in Parkinson’s disease , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  Nicola J. Ray,et al.  The role of the subthalamic nucleus in response inhibition: Evidence from deep brain stimulation for Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  Julie Duque,et al.  Role of corticospinal suppression during motor preparation. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[16]  W. Byblow,et al.  Intracortical inhibition during volitional inhibition of prepared action. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  Sebastiaan Overeem,et al.  Expectancy Induces Dynamic Modulation of Corticospinal Excitability , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  D. Willshaw,et al.  A massively connected subthalamic nucleus leads to the generation of widespread pulses , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  C. Capaday,et al.  Input-output properties and gain changes in the human corticospinal pathway , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Influence of Uncertainty and Surprise on Human Corticospinal Excitability during Preparation for Action , 2008, Current Biology.

[22]  Franck Vidal,et al.  The dual nature of time preparation: neural activation and suppression revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the motor cortex , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[23]  P. Ashby,et al.  Inhibition in the human motor cortex is reduced just before a voluntary contraction , 1999, Neurology.

[24]  M. W. van der Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses. , 2004, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[25]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Proactive inhibitory control of movement assessed by event-related fMRI , 2008, NeuroImage.

[26]  G. Logan,et al.  Converging Evidence for a Fronto-Basal-Ganglia Network for Inhibitory Control of Action and Cognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Prefrontal‐subcortical dissociations underlying inhibitory control revealed by event‐related fMRI , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  G. Logan,et al.  Models of Response Inhibition in the Stop-signal and Stop-change Paradigms , 2022 .

[29]  A. Aron,et al.  Stop the Presses , 2008, Psychological science.

[30]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Predicting Success: Patterns of Cortical Activation and Deactivation Prior to Response Inhibition , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[31]  Frederick Verbruggen,et al.  Responding with Restraint: What Are the Neurocognitive Mechanisms? , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[32]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[33]  Rajita Sinha,et al.  Subcortical processes of motor response inhibition during a stop signal task , 2008, NeuroImage.

[34]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[35]  B. J. Casey,et al.  Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. , 1997, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[36]  G. Logan,et al.  Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control in Normal Development and Childhood Psychopathology , 1990 .

[37]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses , 2004 .

[38]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal reaction-time task performance: role of prefrontal cortex and subthalamic nucleus. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[39]  W. Byblow,et al.  Selective inhibition of movement. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Processing of global and selective stop signals: application of Donders’ subtraction method to stop-signal task performance , 2010 .

[41]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  Geert J. M. van Boxtel,et al.  Stimulation of the Subthalamic Region Facilitates the Selection and Inhibition of Motor Responses in Parkinson's Disease , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.