Stimulus-driven and goal-driven control over visual selection.

This article explored the extent to which stimulus-driven control over visual selection is modulated by goal-driven factors. Observers searched for a no-onset color target among 3 distractors and signaled its location either manually or with a saccade. Additional distractors appeared either with or without an abrupt onset and were either similar or dissimilar to the target. Abrupt onsets disrupted saccades to the target, especially when they shared the target color. Irrelevant onsets also interfered with the manual responses, but this interference was dependent on the particular type of manual response. Stimulus-driven and contingent capture can occur within a single paradigm, but the extent and nature of these effects depend on the specific response required.

[1]  F. J. Anscombe,et al.  THE TRANSFORMATION OF POISSON, BINOMIAL AND NEGATIVE-BINOMIAL DATA , 1948 .

[2]  M. Saslow Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. , 1967, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[3]  S. Keele Attention demands of memory retrieval. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[4]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  J. Findlay,et al.  The Relationship between Eye Movements and Spatial Attention , 1986, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  J Miller,et al.  The control of attention by abrupt visual onsets and offsets , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  C. Bundesen A theory of visual attention. , 1990, Psychological review.

[10]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[11]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: An analysis of the gap effect , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[13]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  S. Yantis Stimulus-driven attentional capture and attentional control settings. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  S. Yantis,et al.  Stimulus-driven attentional capture: evidence from equiluminant visual objects. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Space and selective attention , 1994 .

[17]  J. Theeuwes Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  R. Klein,et al.  Does Oculomotor Readiness Mediate Cognitive Control of Visual-Attention - Revisited , 1994 .

[19]  R W Remington,et al.  The structure of attentional control: contingent attentional capture by apparent motion, abrupt onset, and color. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[21]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[22]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[23]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  B. Dosher,et al.  The role of attention in the programming of saccades , 1995, Vision Research.

[25]  Bradley S. Gibson,et al.  THE MASKING ACCOUNT OF ATTENTIONAL CAPTURE : A REPLY TO YANTIS AND JONIDES(1996) , 1996 .

[26]  B. Gibson,et al.  Visual quality and attentional capture: a challenge to the special role of abrupt onsets. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[27]  H. Deubel,et al.  Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism , 1996, Vision Research.

[28]  Raymond M. Klein,et al.  The Magnitude of the Fixation Offset Effect with Endogenously and Exogenously Controlled Saccades , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[29]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Visual marking: prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. , 1997, Psychological review.

[30]  Arthur F. Kramer,et al.  Offset transients modulate attentional capture by sudden onsets , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[31]  D. Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Activity in Monkey Superior Colliculus Related to the Initiation of Saccadic Eye Movements , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  Heike Weber,et al.  Effects of stimulus conditions on the performance of antisaccades in man , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[33]  S. Yantis,et al.  Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. , 1997, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  J. Richards Cognitive neuroscience of attention : a developmental perspective , 1998 .

[35]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[36]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[37]  C. Pierrot-Deseilligny,et al.  Cortical control of saccades , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional control during visual search: the effect of irrelevant singletons. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[39]  R. Walker,et al.  A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[40]  Steven P. Tipper,et al.  Visual search and target-directed action , 1999 .

[41]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system , 1999, Vision Research.

[42]  K. Cave,et al.  Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[43]  D P Munoz,et al.  Role of Primate Superior Colliculus in Preparation and Execution of Anti-Saccades and Pro-Saccades , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  A. Gellatly,et al.  Do equiluminant object onsets capture visual attention? , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[45]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons , 2000, Vision Research.

[48]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  A. Sereno,et al.  Inhibition of return in manual and saccadic response systems , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[50]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[51]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Composition and topographic organization of signals sent from the frontal eye field to the superior colliculus. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[52]  A. Gellatly,et al.  Accuracy of target detection in new-object and old-object displays. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[53]  D. Simons Attentional capture and inattentional blindness , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Interaction of the frontal eye field and superior colliculus for saccade generation. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  Charles L. Folk,et al.  Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention? , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[56]  R. Douglas,et al.  Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Orienting of attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.