Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task.

Theories about the functional relevance of consciousness commonly posit that higher order cognitive control functions, such as response inhibition, require consciousness. To test this assertion, the authors designed a masked stop-signal paradigm to examine whether response inhibition could be triggered and initiated by masked stop signals, which inform participants to stop an action they have begun. In 2 experiments, masked stop signals were observed to occasionally result in full response inhibition as well as to yield a slow down in the speed of responses that were not inhibited. The magnitude of this subliminally triggered response time slowing effect correlated with the efficiency measure (stop signal reaction time) of response inhibition across participants. Thus, response inhibition can be triggered unconsciously-more so in individuals who are good inhibitors and under conditions that are associated with efficient response inhibition. These results indicate that in contradiction to common theorizing, inhibitory control processes can operate outside awareness.

[1]  W. Singer,et al.  Better than conscious? : decision making, the human mind, and implications for institutions , 2008 .

[2]  M. Brass,et al.  Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[3]  M. Brass,et al.  To Do or Not to Do: The Neural Signature of Self-Control , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[4]  C. Kennard,et al.  Human Medial Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconscious Inhibition of Voluntary Action , 2007, Neuron.

[5]  R. Passingham,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Cognitive Control System in the Human Prefrontal Cortex , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Leanne Boucher,et al.  Influence of history on saccade countermanding performance in humans and macaque monkeys , 2007, Vision Research.

[7]  A. Represa,et al.  Greater Disruption Due to Failure of Inhibitory Control on an Ambiguous Distractor , 2006, Science.

[8]  K. Yu,et al.  Attentional modulation of sensorimotor processes in the absence of perceptual awareness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[9]  O. Neumann,et al.  Intentions determine the effect of invisible metacontrast-masked primes: evidence for top-down contingencies in a peripheral cuing task. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The Neurophysiology of Response Competition: Motor Cortex Activation and Inhibition following Subliminal Response Priming , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[11]  B. Libet Do we have free will , 2005 .

[12]  Deborah E. Hannula,et al.  Imaging implicit perception: promise and pitfalls , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[13]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Decoupling stimulus duration from brightness in metacontrast masking: data and models. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Evidence for an Error Monitoring Deficit in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder , 2004, Journal of abnormal child psychology.

[15]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  Conflict, consciousness, and control , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  M. W. Molen,et al.  Developmental trends in simple and selective inhibition of compatible and incompatible responses , 2004 .

[17]  R. Passingham,et al.  Attention to Intention , 2004, Science.

[18]  Ulrich Ansorge,et al.  Top–Down Contingencies of Nonconscious Priming Revealed by Dual–Task Interference , 2004, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[19]  M. Leboyer,et al.  Conscious and subliminal conflicts in normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia: The role of the anterior cingulate , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  M. Eimer,et al.  Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[21]  Katya Rubia,et al.  Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection , 2003, NeuroImage.

[22]  Yves Rossetti,et al.  Abstraction from a sensori-motor perspective: can we get a quick hold on simple perception? , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[23]  J. Hoffmann,et al.  Conscious control over the content of unconscious cognition , 2003, Cognition.

[24]  J. Schwarzbach,et al.  Different time courses for visual perception and action priming , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[25]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[26]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Long-term semantic memory versus contextual memory in unconscious number processing. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[27]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[28]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[29]  T. Robbins,et al.  Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[31]  B. Baars,et al.  The conscious access hypothesis: origins and recent evidence , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  B. Libet,et al.  Conscious intention and brain activity. , 2001 .

[33]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: evidence from an antisaccade task. , 2001, Psychophysiology.

[34]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework , 2001, Cognition.

[35]  J. Eastwood,et al.  Perception without awareness: perspectives from cognitive psychology , 2001, Cognition.

[36]  M. Damian,et al.  Congruity effects evoked by subliminally presented primes: automaticity rather than semantic processing. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  M. Desmurget,et al.  An ‘automatic pilot’ for the hand in human posterior parietal cortex: toward reinterpreting optic ataxia , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[38]  Richard L. Abrams,et al.  Parts Outweigh the Whole (Word) in Unconscious Analysis of Meaning , 2000, Psychological science.

[39]  Martina Rieger,et al.  Inhibitory after‐effects in the stop signal paradigm , 1999 .

[40]  M. Eimer Facilitatory and inhibitory effects of masked prime stimuli on motor activation and behavioural performance. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[41]  J. Hohnsbein,et al.  ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[42]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Imaging unconscious semantic priming , 1998, Nature.

[44]  J. Mattingley,et al.  Parietal neglect and visual awareness , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  G. Francis Cortical Dynamics of Lateral Inhibition: Metacontrast Masking , 1997 .

[46]  A. Cowey,et al.  Blindsight in man and monkey. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[48]  T. Carr,et al.  Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory and Language , 1994 .

[49]  Anthony J. Marcel,et al.  Consciousness in Contemporary Science , 1992 .

[50]  L. Jacoby A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory , 1991 .

[51]  T Bachmann,et al.  Microgenesis as traced by the transient paired-forms paradigm. , 1989, Acta psychologica.

[52]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[53]  Curtis A. Gleason,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[54]  D. Raab,et al.  Reaction time to stimuli masked by metacontrast. , 1962, Journal of experimental psychology.

[55]  HighWire Press Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.

[56]  S. Dehaene Conscious and Nonconscious Processes:Distinct Forms of Evidence Accumulation? , 2011 .

[57]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  R. H. Baayen,et al.  The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM) , 1996 .

[59]  O. Neumann Direct parameter specification and the concept of perception , 1990, Psychological research.

[60]  Carlo Umiltà,et al.  The control operations of consciousness. , 1988 .