Comparative Prey-Attack Studies in Newborn Snakes of the Genus Thamnophis

Garter snakes (Thamnophis) from five species (including three subspecies of one form) were tested several days after birth with water extracts of at least 12 small animals (1.5 g animal to 10 ccm warm distilled water). The 12 prey animals included three species of earthworms and three of fish, a salamander and its larva, a frog, a leech, a slug and a baby mouse. A distilled water swab elicited tongue flicking only, while certain extract swabs resulted in actual prey-attack behavior after at least one tongue flick. A score was given to each extract test using a simple formula based upon tongue flick frequency and attack latency. Differences and similarities between the species were found and are discussed in relation to the actual feeding preferences in nature and captivity. For example, the aquatic Thamnophis elegans aquaticus attacked only the extracts made from the salamander larva, the frog, and the three fish; Thamnophis sirtalis also attacked the leech and the three earthworm extracts. It is suggested that the perceptual selectivity shown by naive snakes is an evolutionary response to present and past ecological conditions. The ability of newborn snakes to rapidly acquire a food avoidance response was also demonstrated.

[1]  Robert C. Stebbins,et al.  A field guide to western reptiles and amphibians : field marks of all species in western North America , 1998 .

[2]  G. Burghardt,et al.  Factors influencing the chemical release of prey attack in newborn snakes. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[3]  M. Dix Snake Food Preference: Innate Intraspecific Geographic Variation , 1968, Science.

[4]  G. Burghardt Chemical-Cue Preferences of Inexperienced Snakes: Comparative Aspects , 1967, Science.

[5]  Philip Smith,et al.  The Amphibians and Reptiles of Illinois , 1961 .

[6]  L. Brower,et al.  Experimental Studies of Mimicry. 5. The Reactions of Toads (Bufo terrestris) to Bumblebees (Bombus americanorum) and Their Robberfly Mimics (Mallophora bomboides), with a Discussion of Aggressive Mimicry , 1960, The American Naturalist.

[7]  Roger Conant,et al.  A Field Guide to Reptiles and Amphibians , 1959 .

[8]  C. Carpenter Comparative Ecology of the Common Garter Snake (Thamnophis s. sirtalis), the Ribbon Snake (Thamnophis s. sauritus), and Butler's Garter Snake (Thamnophis butleri) in Mixed Populations , 1952 .

[9]  D. Wolfle,et al.  A Learning Experiment with Snakes , 1940 .

[10]  K. Schmidt Herpetological Evidence for the Postglacial Eastward Extension of the Steppe in North America , 1938 .

[11]  W. N. Kellogg,et al.  Maze learning in water snakes. , 1936 .

[12]  G. K. Noble,et al.  The Aggregation Behavior of Storeria Dekayi and Other Snakes, with Especial Reference to the Sense Organs Involved , 1936 .

[13]  Arthur G. Brown Variations and Genetic Relationships of the Garter Snakes , 1908 .

[14]  G. Burghardt,et al.  ON THE NATURE OF CHEMICAL FOOD SIGN STIMULI FOR NEWBORN SNAKES , 1968 .

[15]  F. Crawford,et al.  Runway behavior of the gray rat snake with food and water reinforcement , 1966 .

[16]  Charles E. Holmes,et al.  Escape conditioning in snakes employing vibratory stimulation , 1966 .

[17]  R. C. Stebbins Amphibians and Reptiles of Western North America , 1954 .

[18]  N. Tinbergen,et al.  On the Stimulus Situation Releasing the Begging Response in the Newly Hatched Herring Gull Chick (Larus Argentatus Argentatus Pont.) , 1951 .

[19]  W. S. Wilde The role of Jacobson's organ in the feeding reaction of the common garter snake, Thamnophis sirtalis sirtalis (Linn.) , 1938 .