Asymmetries in mobbing behaviour and correlated intensity during predator mobbing by nuthatches, chickadees and titmice

In mixed-species foraging flocks of forest birds, one or a few nuclear species frequently produce alarm calls and are followed by other species in the flock. We tested the hypothesis that similar asymmetries may exist in a second interspecific social context, multispecies mobbing behaviour. We examined mobbing behaviour evoked by an eastern screech-owl, Megascops asio, model and playback in two nuclear species, Carolina chickadees, Poecile carolinensis, and tufted titmice, Baeolophus bicolor, and in a species that follows them, the white-breasted nuthatch, Sitta carolinensis. Asymmetries in mobbing were not the same as those in mixed-species flocks. Nuthatches and chickadees mobbed with greater frequency and intensity compared to titmice, which remained at greater distances from the owl model and vocalized less frequently. We also tested for the existence and nature of potential interspecific vocal information flow during mobbing. Chickadee and nuthatch calling rates were positively correlated, as were chickadee and titmouse calling rates. Nuthatches and titmice rarely mobbed simultaneously. These results suggest the existence of positive feedback among species' mobbing intensity during a multispecies mobbing association as opposed to heterospecific vocal interference or a lack of heterospecific influence. However, randomization simulations showed that this positive feedback was not driven by a particular ‘nuclear’ species during mobbing, suggesting that the correlations may result from a mutually interdependent escalation of mobbing intensity.

[1]  P. Greig-Smith THE FORMATION, STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION OF MIXED-SPECIES INSECTIVOROUS BIRD FLOCKS IN WEST AFRICAN SAVANNA WOODLAND , 2008 .

[2]  T. Krama,et al.  Long-lasting mobbing of the pied flycatcher increases the risk of nest predation , 2007 .

[3]  M. Ficken,et al.  A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF PASSERINE MOBBING CALLS , 1996 .

[4]  Erick Greene,et al.  Allometry of Alarm Calls: Black-Capped Chickadees Encode Information About Predator Size , 2005, Science.

[5]  P. Gaddis Mixed Flocks, Accipiters, and Antipredator Behavior , 1980 .

[6]  Karen M Kostan The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[7]  K. Sullivan Information Exploitation By Downy Woodpeckers in Mixed-Species Flocks , 1984 .

[8]  M. Ficken Call Similarities among Mixed Species Flock Associates , 2000 .

[9]  C. Munn Birds that ‘cry wolf’ , 1986, Nature.

[10]  Leonard A. Brennan,et al.  Ecology and Behavior of Chickadees and Titmice: An Integrated Approach , 2007 .

[11]  T. Krama,et al.  Experimental evidence of reciprocal altruism in the pied flycatcher , 2008, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[12]  T. Sordahl The risks of avian mobbing and distraction behavior : an anecdotal review , 1990 .

[13]  P. Doran,et al.  Avian Mobbing Response is Restricted by Territory Boundaries: Experimental Evidence from Two Species of Forest Warblers , 2005 .

[14]  R. Bshary,et al.  Red colobus and Diana monkeys provide mutual protection against predators , 1997, Animal Behaviour.

[15]  A. Flasskamp The Adaptive Significance of Avian Mobbing V. An Experimental Test of the ‘Move On’ Hypothesis , 2010 .

[16]  Robert Planqué,et al.  Spectral Overlap in Songs and Temporal Avoidance in a Peruvian Bird Assemblage , 2008 .

[17]  H. Brumm,et al.  Signalling through acoustic windows: nightingales avoid interspecific competition by short-term adjustment of song timing , 2006, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[18]  T. Grubb,et al.  Benefits to satellite members in mixed-species foraging groups: an experimental analysis , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[19]  J. Terborgh,et al.  Multi-Species Territoriality in Neotropical Foraging Flocks , 1979 .

[20]  J. Lucas,et al.  A comparative study of avian auditory brainstem responses: correlations with phylogeny and vocal complexity, and seasonal effects , 2002, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[21]  K. M. Kostan The evolution of mutualistic interspecific communication: assessment and management across species. , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[22]  E. Curio,et al.  The adaptive significance of avian mobbing: I. Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions. , 1978 .

[23]  Eben Goodale,et al.  Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  J. Tewksbury,et al.  EFFECT OF NORTHERN PYGMY-OWL (GLAUCIDIUM GNOMA) EYESPOTS ON AVIAN MOBBING , 2003 .

[25]  N. Smith PROVOKED RELEASE OF MOBBING‐A HUNTING TECHNIQUE OF MICRASTUR FALCONS , 2008 .

[26]  Eben Goodale,et al.  Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest , 2008 .

[27]  M. Moynihan,et al.  The organization and probable evolution of some mixed species flocks of Neotropical birds , 1962 .

[28]  W. Latimer,et al.  A Comparative Study of the Songs and Alarm Calls of some Parus Species , 1977 .

[29]  H. L. Bell Composition and Seasonality of Mixed-Species Feeding Flocks of Insectivorous Birds in the Australian Capital Territory , 1980 .

[30]  Richard A. Pettifor,et al.  The effects of avian mobbing on a potential predator, the European kestrel, Falco tinnunculus , 1990, Animal Behaviour.

[31]  D. H. Morse Feeding Behavior and Predator Avoidance in Heterospecific Groups , 1977 .

[32]  Erlend B. Nilsen Antipredator Defences in Birds and Mammals , 2006 .

[33]  Jessica L Owens,et al.  Variation in chick-a-dee calls of tufted titmice, Baeolophus bicolor: note type and individual distinctiveness. , 2007, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[34]  S. Baird,et al.  The birds of North America , 1974 .

[35]  J. Diamond Flocks of Brown and Black New Guinean Birds: A Bicoloured Mixed-species Foraging Association , 1987 .

[36]  K. Zuberbühler,et al.  Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[37]  Jared M. Diamond,et al.  Mixed-species foraging groups , 1981, Nature.

[38]  P. Marler,et al.  Characteristics of Some Animal Calls , 1955, Nature.

[39]  O. Anne,et al.  Dwarf mongoose and hornbill mutualism in the Taru desert, Kenya , 1983, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[40]  M. Jullien,et al.  Multi-species territoriality and dynamic of neotropical forest understorey bird flocks , 1998 .

[41]  M. Ryan,et al.  The mixed-species chorus as public information: túngara frogs eavesdrop on a heterospecific , 2007 .

[42]  P. Sunde,et al.  Diurnal exposure as a risk sensitive behaviour in tawny owls Strix aluco , 2003 .

[43]  D. G. Smith,et al.  The Eastern Screech Owl: Life History, Ecology, and Behavior in the Suburbs and Countryside , 1995 .

[44]  Overview: For crying out loud--dangers and opportunities of communicating in the public domain. , 2002, Journal of comparative psychology.

[45]  M. Child,et al.  Interspecific audience effects on the alarm-calling behaviour of a kleptoparasitic bird , 2007, Biology Letters.

[46]  E. Goodale,et al.  Vocal mimicry by a passerine bird attracts other species involved in mixed-species flocks , 2006, Animal Behaviour.

[47]  S. L. Lima Vigilance and foraging substrate: anti-predatory considerations in a non-standard environment , 1992, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[48]  Jeffrey R. Lucas,et al.  “Information” and the chick-a-dee call: Communicating with a complex vocal system , 2007 .

[49]  P. Sunde,et al.  Exposure affects the risk of an owl being mobbed / experimental evidence , 2006 .

[50]  Marc D. Hauser,et al.  How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: The role of experience. , 1988 .

[51]  D. H. Morse,et al.  Ecological Aspects of Some Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks of Birds , 1970 .

[52]  I. Krams Communication in crested tits and the risk of predation , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[53]  R. Greenberg Birds of Many Feathers: The Formation and Structure of Mixed Species Flocks of Forest Birds , 2001 .

[54]  W. Shriner Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[55]  D. Blumstein,et al.  Heterospecific eavesdropping in a nonsocial species , 2008 .

[56]  C. Munn Permanent Canopy and Understory Flocks in Amazonia: Species Composition and Population Density , 1985 .

[57]  R. Oda,et al.  Interspecific Responses of Ringtailed Lemurs to Playback of Antipredator Alarm Calls Given by Verreaux's Sifakas , 2010 .

[58]  T. Grubb,et al.  An Experimental Analysis of Mixed‐Species Flocking in Birds of Deciduous Woodland , 1985 .

[59]  Christine R. Hurd Interspecific attraction to the mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus) , 1996, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[60]  Laurie L. Bloomfield,et al.  Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. III: The chick-a-dee call of the Carolina chickadee (Poecile carolinensis) , 2005 .

[61]  F. Gehlbach,et al.  Mobbing of eastern screech-owls: predatory cues, risk to mobbers and degree of threat , 1995 .

[62]  A. Desrochers,et al.  Playbacks of mobbing calls of Black-capped Chickadees help estimate the abundance of forest birds in winter , 2002 .

[63]  Eben Goodale,et al.  ALARM CALLING IN SRI LANKAN MIXED-SPECIES BIRD FLOCKS , 2005 .

[64]  U. Ernst,et al.  Cultural Transmission of Enemy Recognition: One Function of Mobbing , 1978, Science.

[65]  Benjamin J. Pitcher,et al.  A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other's aerial alarm calls , 2007 .

[66]  Kimberly Maute,et al.  HETEROSPECIFIC FACILITATION OF FOREST-BOUNDARY CROSSING BY MOBBING UNDERSTORY BIRDS IN NORTH-CENTRAL FLORIDA , 2004 .

[67]  E. Frankenberg,et al.  The Adaptive Significance of Avian Mobbing , 2010 .

[68]  U. Ramakrishnan,et al.  Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalizations by bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[69]  R. Planqué,et al.  Spectral overlap in songs and temporal overlap in a Peruvian bird assemblage , 2008 .

[70]  S. Altmann AVIAN MOBBING BEHAVIOR AND PREDATOR RECOGNITION , 1956 .

[71]  C. Fichtel Reciprocal recognition of sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) and redfronted lemur (Eulemur fulvus rufus) alarm calls , 2004, Animal Cognition.

[72]  T. Getty,et al.  Cooperative nest defence in red–winged blackbirds: reciprocal altruism, kinship or by–product mutualism? , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[73]  T. Krama,et al.  Cost of mobbing call to breeding pied flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca , 2005 .

[74]  K. Regelmann,et al.  The Behavioural Dynamics of Great Tits (Parus major) Approaching a Predator , 2010 .

[75]  J. Lucas,et al.  Seasonal variation in avian auditory evoked responses to tones: a comparative analysis of Carolina chickadees, tufted titmice, and white-breasted nuthatches , 2007, Journal of Comparative Physiology.

[76]  D. Bell The Eastern Screech Owl: Life History, Ecology, and Behavior in the Suburbs and Countryside.Frederick R. Gehlbach , 1996 .

[77]  K. Regelmann,et al.  Predator Harassment Implies a Real Deadly Risk: A Reply to Hennessy , 2010 .

[78]  A. Poiani,et al.  Predator Harassment: More Evidence on the Deadly Risk , 2010 .

[79]  E. Goodale,et al.  Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rain forest , 2005, Journal of Tropical Ecology.

[80]  Erick Greene,et al.  Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[81]  T. Krama,et al.  Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[82]  P. Leberg,et al.  The Protector Species Hypothesis: Do Black Skimmers Find Refuge from Predators in Gull‐billed Tern Colonies? , 2010 .

[83]  Paul A. Garber,et al.  On the move : how and why animals travel in groups , 2000 .

[84]  C. Pavey,et al.  Effects of avian mobbing on roost use and diet of powerful owls, Ninox strenua , 1998, Animal Behaviour.

[85]  Eberhard Curio,et al.  The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. III. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: Cross-species tutoring and properties of learning , 1980, Animal Behaviour.

[86]  G. Klump,et al.  Acoustic Behaviour of Birds and Mammals in the Predator Context; I. Factors Affecting the Structure of Alarm Signals. II. The Functional Significance and Evolution of Alarm Signals , 2010 .

[87]  P. Slater,et al.  Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[88]  T. Grubb,et al.  Functional Roles in Mixed-Species Foraging Flocks: A Field Manipulation , 1999 .

[89]  M. Mönkkönen,et al.  Responses by breeding birds to heterospecific song and mobbing call playbacks under varying predation risk , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[90]  C. Fitzgibbon Mixed-species grouping in Thomson's and Grant's gazelles: the antipredator benefits , 1990, Animal Behaviour.