Individual differences in virtual reality: Are spatial presence and spatial ability linked?

One aim of virtual reality technology is to immerse the user in a digital environment that is distinct from physical reality. Feeling spatially located in this digital environment is central to the experience and is more formally known as spatial presence. Experiences of spatial presence differ between individuals; prominent theories assume that these differences may, in part, be explained by differences in more general spatial abilities. Whilst there is some support for this claim with desktop systems, there is currently no direct empirical evidence to support this with more immersive technologies such as head-mounted displays (HMDs). In this study, participants completed three different measures of spatial ability before experiencing two virtual environments. These measures included a self-report of visuospatial imagery; the mental rotations test; and a test of topographical memory. After completing the measures, participants briefly experienced a virtual city and a virtual train ride through a HMD. The user’s head movements were tracked, and visual displays were updated to give the sense of a full 360° environment. After each experience, the participants reported how present they felt and the extent to which they had a mental model of the environment. Self-reports of imagery were positively correlated with reports of spatial presence, consistent with the previous literature. However, spatial presence was not related to performance on either of the more objective tests. Whilst this provides confirmatory evidence that self-reports of imagery can predict presence, it is still unclear which more basic spatial abilities, if any, could underlie this relationship.

[1]  V. Seagroatt An introduction to medical statistics (2nd ed.) , 1996 .

[2]  Matthew Lombard,et al.  At the Heart of It All: The Concept of Presence , 2006 .

[3]  David B. Kaber,et al.  Telepresence , 1998, Hum. Factors.

[4]  Jeremy N. Bailenson,et al.  How Immersive Is Enough? A Meta-Analysis of the Effect of Immersive Technology on User Presence , 2016 .

[5]  W. Wirth,et al.  Structural Equation Modeling of Spatial Presence: The Influence of Cognitive Processes and Traits , 2012 .

[6]  S. Vandenberg,et al.  Mental Rotations, a Group Test of Three-Dimensional Spatial Visualization , 1978, Perceptual and motor skills.

[7]  Robert S. Kennedy,et al.  Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. , 1993 .

[8]  Yun Ling,et al.  The relationship between individual characteristics and experienced presence , 2013, Comput. Hum. Behav..

[9]  Christoph Klimmt,et al.  The Spatial Presence Experience Scale (SPES) , 2016, J. Media Psychol. Theor. Methods Appl..

[10]  T. Hartley,et al.  An association between human hippocampal volume and topographical memory in healthy young adults , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[11]  N. Burgess,et al.  The hippocampus is required for short‐term topographical memory in humans , 2007, Hippocampus.

[12]  José Gutiérrez-Maldonado,et al.  Influence of personality and individual abilities on the sense of presence experienced in anxiety triggering virtual environments , 2010, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[13]  Jonathan Steuer,et al.  Defining virtual reality: dimensions determining telepresence , 1992 .

[14]  Martin Bland,et al.  An Introduction to Medical Statistics , 1987 .

[15]  S. Kosslyn Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery Debate , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[16]  Christoph Klimmt,et al.  A Process Model of the Formation of Spatial Presence Experiences , 2007 .

[17]  B. Laeng,et al.  A Redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test - Different Versions and Factors That Affect Performance , 1995, Brain and Cognition.

[18]  Hilla Peretz,et al.  The , 1966 .