The Plausible Impossible: Causal Constraints on Magical Reasoning

A common intuition, often captured in fiction, is that some impossible events (e.g., levitating a stone) are “more impossible” than others (e.g., levitating a feather). We investigated the source of this intuition, hypothesizing that graded notions of impossibility arise from explanatory considerations logically precluded by the violation at hand but still taken into account. Studies 1-2 involved college undergraduates (n = 192), and Study 3 involved preschool-aged children (n = 32). In Study 1, participants saw pairs of magical events (spells) that violated one of 18 causal principles—six physical, six biological, and six psychological—and were asked to indicate which spell would be more difficult to learn. Both spells violated the same causal principle but differed in their relation to a subsidiary principle. Participants’ judgments of spell difficulty honored the subsidiary principle, even when participants were given the option of judging the two spells equally difficult. Study 2 replicated the effects of Study 1 with Likert-type ratings, and Study 3 replicated those effects in children. Taken together, these findings suggest that events that defy causal explanation are interpreted in terms of explanatory considerations that hold in the absence of such violations.

[1]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Attributes of God: Conceptual Foundations of a Foundational Belief , 2016, Cogn. Sci..

[2]  Scott Atran,et al.  Memory and Mystery: The Cultural Selection of Minimally Counterintuitive Narratives , 2006, Cogn. Sci..

[3]  R. D. Kavanaugh,et al.  Young children's comprehension of pretend episodes: the integration of successive actions. , 1994, Child development.

[4]  J. Woolley,et al.  Thinking about fantasy: are children fundamentally different thinkers and believers from adults? , 1997, Child development.

[5]  J. Woolley,et al.  Preschoolers' Magical Explanations for Violations of Physical, Social, and Mental Laws , 2004 .

[6]  T. Griffiths Revealing ontological commitments by magic , 2015, Cognition.

[7]  Joshua B. Tenenbaum,et al.  Inferring causal networks from observations and interventions , 2003, Cogn. Sci..

[8]  Shaun Nichols,et al.  Imaginative Blocks and Impossibility: An Essay in Modal Psychology , 2006 .

[9]  Rebekah A. Richert,et al.  Preschoolers' quarantining of fantasy stories. , 2011, Child development.

[10]  Joseph Henrich,et al.  The Zeus Problem: Why Representational Content Biases Cannot Explain Faith in Gods , 2010 .

[11]  T. B. Ward Structured Imagination: the Role of Category Structure in Exemplar Generation , 1994, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  Andrew Shtulman,et al.  Cognitive parallels between moral judgment and modal judgment , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[13]  Jonathan D. Lane,et al.  Developing concepts of ordinary and extraordinary communication. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[14]  Gilberte Piéraut-Le Bonniec,et al.  The development of modal reasoning : genesis of necessity and possibility notions , 1980 .

[15]  David M. Sobel,et al.  A theory of causal learning in children: causal maps and Bayes nets. , 2004, Psychological review.

[16]  P. Bloom,et al.  What does Batman think about SpongeBob? Children's understanding of the fantasy/fantasy distinction , 2006, Cognition.

[17]  Andrew Shtulman Variation in the anthropomorphization of supernatural beings and its implications for cognitive theories of religion. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[18]  P. Boyer Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: how evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission , 1998 .

[19]  J. Barrett,et al.  Spreading Non-natural Concepts: The Role of Intuitive Conceptual Structures in Memory and Transmission of Cultural Materials ¤ , 2001 .

[20]  Michael R. Perkins,et al.  Modal Expressions in English , 1983 .

[21]  B. Purzycki The minds of gods: A comparative study of supernatural agency , 2013, Cognition.

[22]  A. Gopnik,et al.  The power of possibility: causal learning, counterfactual reasoning, and pretend play , 2012, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[23]  Frank C. Keil,et al.  The More Things Change ...: Metamorphoses and Conceptual Structure , 1985, Cogn. Sci..

[24]  Andrew Shtulman The development of possibility judgment within and across domains , 2009 .

[25]  M. Afzal Upal,et al.  Memory, Mystery and Coherence: Does the Presence of 2‐3 Counterintuitive Concepts Predict Cultural Success of a Narrative? , 2011 .

[26]  Ustin,et al.  Spreading Non-natural Concepts : The Role of Intuitive Conceptual Structures in Memory and Transmission of Cultural Materials ¤ , 2004 .

[27]  S. Carey,et al.  Improbable or impossible? How children reason about the possibility of extraordinary events. , 2007, Child development.

[28]  D. Weisberg,et al.  What Belongs in a Fictional World , 2009 .

[29]  K. Rosengren,et al.  Seeing is believing: children's explanations of commonplace, magical, and extraordinary transformations. , 1994, Child development.

[30]  M. Chandler,et al.  Surprising, magical and miraculous turns of events: Children's reactions to violations of their early theories of mind and matter , 1994 .

[31]  Caren M. Walker,et al.  Learning to learn from stories: children's developing sensitivity to the causal structure of fictional worlds. , 2015, Child development.

[32]  Qi Wang,et al.  A Comparison of American and Nepalese Children's Concepts of Freedom of Choice and Social Constraint , 2013, Cogn. Sci..