Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons

For more than 2 decades, researchers have debated the nature of cognitive control in the guidance of visual attention. Stimulus-driven theories claim that salient stimuli automatically capture attention, whereas goal-driven theories propose that an individual’s attentional control settings determine whether salient stimuli capture attention. In the current study, we tested a hybrid account called the signal suppression hypothesis, which claims that all stimuli automatically generate a salience signal but that this signal can be actively suppressed by top-down attentional mechanisms. Previous behavioral and electrophysiological research has shown that participants can suppress covert shifts of attention to salient-but-irrelevant color singletons. In this study, we used eye-tracking methods to determine whether participants can also suppress overt shifts of attention to irrelevant singletons. We found that under conditions that promote active suppression of the irrelevant singletons, overt attention was less likely to be directed toward the salient distractors than toward nonsalient distractors. This result provides direct evidence that people can suppress salient-but-irrelevant singletons below baseline levels.

[1]  Frederick S. Barrett,et al.  Developmental changes in visual short-term memory in infancy: evidence from eye-tracking , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[2]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  No capture outside the attentional window , 2010, Vision Research.

[3]  R. Remington,et al.  Selectivity in distraction by irrelevant featural singletons: evidence for two forms of attentional capture. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  B. Breitmeyer,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Jeremy M. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 4.0: Current Progress With a Model of Visual Search , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[6]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[7]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention , 2000 .

[8]  J. Theeuwes Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[9]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[10]  David M. Groppe,et al.  Mass univariate analysis of event-related brain potentials/fields I: a critical tutorial review. , 2011, Psychophysiology.

[11]  D. Simons,et al.  Moving and looming stimuli capture attention , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[12]  J. Mordkoff,et al.  Contingent Capture at a very short SOA: Evidence against Rapid Disengagement , 2007 .

[13]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  The distractor positivity (Pd) signals lowering of attentional priority: evidence from event-related potentials and individual differences. , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Daniel B. Vatterott,et al.  Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection , 2012, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  The role of magnocellular signals in oculomotor attentional capture. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[17]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  Enhanced distraction by magnocellular salience signals in schizophrenia , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  S Yantis,et al.  Dividing attention between color and shape: Evidence of coactivation , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  John M. Gaspar,et al.  Suppression of Salient Objects Prevents Distraction in Visual Search , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  N. Clark,et al.  Direct Evidence , 1934 .

[21]  James T. Townsend,et al.  A note on the identifiability of parallel and serial processes , 1971 .

[22]  Geoffrey F Woodman,et al.  Serial deployment of attention during visual search. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[23]  P. Reuter-Lorenz,et al.  The reduction of saccadic latency by prior offset of the fixation point: An analysis of the gap effect , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.

[24]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  J. R. Mounts,et al.  Attentional capture by abrupt onsets and feature singletons produces inhibitory surrounds , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[26]  H. Egeth,et al.  Attention on autopilot: Past experience and attentional set , 2006 .

[27]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[28]  J. Theeuwes Perceptual selectivity for color and form , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[29]  K. Cave,et al.  Spatial Attention in Visual Search for Features and Feature Conjunctions , 1995 .

[30]  J. Townsend Serial vs. Parallel Processing: Sometimes They Look like Tweedledum and Tweedledee but they can (and Should) be Distinguished , 1990 .

[31]  Howard E Egeth,et al.  The ignoring paradox: Cueing distractor features leads first to selection, then to inhibition of to-be-ignored items , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[32]  R. Oostenveld,et al.  Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data , 2007, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[33]  Robert Desimone,et al.  Parallel and Serial Neural Mechanisms for Visual Search in Macaque Area V4 , 2005, Science.

[34]  Eric Ruthruff,et al.  The problem of latent attentional capture: Easy visual search conceals capture by task-irrelevant abrupt onsets. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[35]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  The size of an attentional window modulates attentional capture by color singletons , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[36]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  LIP responses to a popout stimulus are reduced if it is overtly ignored , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[37]  Preeti Verghese,et al.  The psychophysics of visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[38]  J. Hoffman,et al.  The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[39]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[40]  Steven J Luck,et al.  Active suppression of distractors that match the contents of visual working memory , 2011, Visual cognition.

[41]  Eric Ruthruff,et al.  Breaking through the attentional window: Capture by abrupt onsets versus color singletons , 2012, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[42]  J. Palmer Attention in Visual Search: Distinguishing Four Causes of a Set-Size Effect , 1995 .

[43]  S. Yantis,et al.  Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention , 1988, Perception & psychophysics.

[44]  J. Theeuwes Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[45]  J. Townsend Serial and within-stage independent parallel model equivalence on the minimum completion time ☆ , 1976 .

[46]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[47]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Involuntary Attentional Capture is Determined by Task Set: Evidence from Event-related Brain Potentials , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[48]  H. Egeth,et al.  Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[50]  S. Vecera,et al.  Learned Control Over Distraction Is Disrupted in Amnesia , 2013, Psychological science.

[51]  J. Henderson Human gaze control during real-world scene perception , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[52]  E. Vogel,et al.  Individual Differences in Recovery Time From Attentional Capture , 2011, Psychological science.

[53]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture with static singletons , 2003, Perception & psychophysics.

[54]  Ali Jannati,et al.  Tracking target and distractor processing in fixed-feature visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  S. Hutton,et al.  The antisaccade task as a research tool in psychopathology: a critical review. , 2006, Psychophysiology.

[56]  Joy J Geng,et al.  Attentional capture by a perceptually salient non-target facilitates target processing through inhibition and rapid rejection. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[57]  Michael Zehetleitner,et al.  Top-down control of attention: it's gradual, practice-dependent, and hierarchically organized. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[58]  Joshua B. Ewen,et al.  Inhibition Drives Early Feature-Based Attention , 2014, Psychological science.

[59]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[60]  Toshiaki Imada,et al.  Theta brain rhythms index perceptual narrowing in infant speech perception , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[61]  Frank E. Ritter,et al.  The Rise of Cognitive Architectures , 2007, Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems.

[62]  Carly J. Leonard,et al.  Direct Evidence for Active Suppression of Salient-but-Irrelevant Sensory Inputs , 2015, Psychological science.

[63]  David J. Prime,et al.  Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[64]  Shu-Chieh Wu,et al.  Characteristics of covert and overt visual orienting: Evidence from attentional and oculomotor capture. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[65]  Vincent Di Lollo,et al.  Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[66]  E. Ruthruff,et al.  Susceptible to distraction: Children lack top-down control over spatial attention capture , 2015, Psychonomic bulletin & review.