Mode-dependent control of human walking and running as revealed by split-belt locomotor adaptation

ABSTRACT Here, we investigate the association of neural control between walking and running, and in particular, how these two gait modes at different velocities are controlled by the central nervous system. The subjects were fully adapted by acquiring modified motor patterns to either walk or run on a split-belt treadmill driven in split mode (asymmetry in the velocities of two belts at 1.0 and 2.0 m s−1). Subsequently, we tested how the adaptation affected walking and running at three different velocities in the tied mode (equal belt velocities). At 0.75 m s−1, we found a preference to walk, at 1.50 m s−1, there was a preference to both walk and run, and at a velocity of 2.25 m s−1 there was a preference to run. Both walking and running on the split belt resulted in the emergence of a significant aftereffect (asymmetrical movement) at all of the velocities tested when walking after adapting to walk and running after adapting to run. However, for contrasting modes (i.e. running after adapting to walk and walking after adapting to run), such aftereffects were far less evident at all velocities; thus showing only limited transfer across gait modes. The results demonstrate a clear mode dependency in the neural control of human walking and running. In addition, only for walking, was there a degree of velocity dependency. Summary: Walking and running are not simply dependent on velocity, but are controlled by the CNS as two completely distinct forms of locomotion in humans.

[1]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Modular Control of Limb Movements during Human Locomotion , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  O. Kiehn,et al.  Dual-mode operation of neuronal networks involved in left–right alternation , 2013, Nature.

[4]  Daniel P. Ferris,et al.  Effect of reduced gravity on the preferred walk-run transition speed. , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[5]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Split-belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[6]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Motor patterns in human walking and running. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Julia T. Choi,et al.  Adaptation reveals independent control networks for human walking , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  W. H. Warren,et al.  Why change gaits? Dynamics of the walk-run transition. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  Noritaka Kawashima,et al.  Limited Transfer of Newly Acquired Movement Patterns across Walking and Running in Humans , 2012, PloS one.

[10]  W. B. Lindquist,et al.  Continuous shifts in the active set of spinal interneurons during changes in locomotor speed , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  A. Thorstensson,et al.  Adaptations to changing speed in human locomotion: speed of transition between walking and running. , 1987, Acta physiologica Scandinavica.

[12]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Spatiotemporal organization of α‐motoneuron activity in the human spinal cord during different gaits and gait transitions , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[13]  S. Grillner,et al.  The adaptation to speed in human locomotion , 1979, Brain Research.

[14]  Motoki Kouzaki,et al.  Identification of muscle synergies associated with gait transition in humans , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[15]  A Hreljac,et al.  Determinants of the gait transition speed during human locomotion: kinetic factors , 1993 .

[16]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  Gait transitions in simulated reduced gravity. , 2011, Journal of applied physiology.

[17]  D. O'Malley,et al.  Locomotor repertoire of the larval zebrafish: swimming, turning and prey capture. , 2000, The Journal of experimental biology.