The Subthalamic Nucleus Contributes to Post-error Slowing

pFC is proposed to implement cognitive control via directed “top–down” influence over behavior. But how is this feat achieved? The virtue of such a descriptive model is contingent on a mechanistic understanding of how motor execution is altered in specific circumstances. In this report, we provide evidence that the well-known phenomenon of slowed RTs following mistakes (post-error slowing) is directly influenced by the degree of subthalamic nucleus (STN) activity. The STN is proposed to act as a brake on motor execution following conflict or errors, buying time so a more cautious response can be made on the next trial. STN local field potentials from nine Parkinson disease patients undergoing deep brain stimulation surgery were recorded while they performed a response conflict task. In a 2.5- to 5-Hz frequency range previously associated with conflict and error processing, the degree phase consistency preceding the response was associated with increasingly slower RTs specifically following errors. These findings provide compelling evidence that post-error slowing is in part mediated by a corticosubthalamic “hyperdirect” pathway for increased response caution.

[1]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  M. Berger,et al.  High Gamma Power Is Phase-Locked to Theta Oscillations in Human Neocortex , 2006, Science.

[3]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[4]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role for Subthalamic Nucleus Neurons in Switching from Automatic to Controlled Eye Movement , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  Timothy Edward John Behrens,et al.  Triangulating a Cognitive Control Network Using Diffusion-Weighted Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) and Functional MRI , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  R. Poldrack,et al.  Cortical and Subcortical Contributions to Stop Signal Response Inhibition: Role of the Subthalamic Nucleus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Oxford handbook of event-related potential components. , 2011 .

[8]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[9]  A. Nambu,et al.  Functional significance of the cortico–subthalamo–pallidal ‘hyperdirect’ pathway , 2002, Neuroscience Research.

[10]  J. Dostrovsky,et al.  Beta oscillatory activity in the subthalamic nucleus and its relation to dopaminergic response in Parkinson's disease. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  P. Brown,et al.  New insights into the relationship between dopamine, beta oscillations and motor function , 2011, Trends in Neurosciences.

[12]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Prelude to and Resolution of an Error: EEG Phase Synchrony Reveals Cognitive Control Dynamics during Action Monitoring , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: neural substrates of parallel processing , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[14]  Ned Jenkinson,et al.  Subthalamic Nucleus Local Field Potential Activity during the Eriksen Flanker Task Reveals a Novel Role for Theta Phase during Conflict Monitoring , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[16]  Sara Marceglia,et al.  What neurophysiological recordings tell us about cognitive and behavioral functions of the human subthalamic nucleus , 2011, Expert review of neurotherapeutics.

[17]  Peter Brown,et al.  Dopaminergic therapy promotes lateralized motor activity in the subthalamic area in Parkinson's disease. , 2007, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[18]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Role of the Medial Frontal Cortex in Cognitive Control , 2004, Science.

[19]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Subthalamic nucleus stimulation influences expression and suppression of impulsive behaviour in Parkinson's disease. , 2010, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[20]  Takahiro Soshi,et al.  Post-error action control is neurobehaviorally modulated under conditions of constant speeded response , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[21]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold Your Horses: Impulsivity, Deep Brain Stimulation, and Medication in Parkinsonism , 2007, Science.

[22]  P. Rabbitt Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[23]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Hold your horses: A dynamic computational role for the subthalamic nucleus in decision making , 2006, Neural Networks.

[24]  P. Brown,et al.  Event-related beta desynchronization in human subthalamic nucleus correlates with motor performance. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[25]  M. Kahana,et al.  Neuronal Activity in the Human Subthalamic Nucleus Encodes Decision Conflict during Action Selection , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  M. Jahanshahi,et al.  The subthalamic nucleus is involved in successful inhibition in the stop-signal task: A local field potential study in Parkinson's disease , 2013, Experimental Neurology.

[27]  S. Luck,et al.  The Oxford handbook of event-related potential components , 2011 .

[28]  Pierre-Louis Bazin,et al.  Cortico-subthalamic white matter tract strength predicts interindividual efficacy in stopping a motor response , 2012, NeuroImage.

[29]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. , 1998, Science.

[30]  Peter Brown,et al.  The subthalamic nucleus, oscillations, and conflict , 2015, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[31]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  The Diffusion Decision Model: Theory and Data for Two-Choice Decision Tasks , 2008, Neural Computation.

[32]  Michael X. Cohen,et al.  Single-Trial Regression Elucidates the Role of Prefrontal Theta Oscillations in Response Conflict , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[33]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  A computational model of inhibitory control in frontal cortex and basal ganglia. , 2011, Psychological review.

[34]  R. Duncan Luce,et al.  Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization , 1986 .

[35]  E. Wagenmakers,et al.  Testing theories of post-error slowing , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[36]  W. Gehring,et al.  Functions of the Medial Frontal Cortex in the Processing of Conflict and Errors , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  J. Mink THE BASAL GANGLIA: FOCUSED SELECTION AND INHIBITION OF COMPETING MOTOR PROGRAMS , 1996, Progress in Neurobiology.

[38]  William J. Gehring,et al.  The Error-Related Negativity (ERN/Ne) , 2011 .

[39]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[40]  Sharna Jamadar,et al.  Adjustments of Response Threshold during Task Switching: A Model-Based Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[41]  A. Engel,et al.  Trial-by-Trial Coupling of Concurrent Electroencephalogram and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Identifies the Dynamics of Performance Monitoring , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  S. Haber,et al.  The Organization of Prefrontal-Subthalamic Inputs in Primates Provides an Anatomical Substrate for Both Functional Specificity and Integration: Implications for Basal Ganglia Models and Deep Brain Stimulation , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  H. Kita,et al.  Excitatory Cortical Inputs to Pallidal Neurons Via the Subthalamic Nucleus in the Monkey , 2000 .

[44]  Ned Jenkinson,et al.  A Role for the Subthalamic Nucleus in Response Inhibition during Conflict , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[45]  John J. B. Allen,et al.  Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.

[46]  Philip G. F. Browning,et al.  Dissociable Components of Rule-Guided Behavior Depend on Distinct Medial and Prefrontal Regions , 2009, Science.

[47]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  Reinforcement-Based Decision Making in Corticostriatal Circuits: Mutual Constraints by Neurocomputational and Diffusion Models , 2012, Neural Computation.

[48]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Review Frontal Cortex and Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Figure 1. Frontal Brain Regions in the Macaque Involved in Reward-guided Learning and Decision-making Finer Grained Anatomical Divisions with Frontal Cortical Systems for Reward-guided Behavior , 2022 .

[49]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[50]  Douglas G Altman,et al.  How to obtain the P value from a confidence interval , 2011, BMJ : British Medical Journal.