12 Motor Control

This chapter gives an overview of basic findings, theoretical approaches, and applied perspectives in the field of motor control. In the first section, the core problem of motor control is introduced and two fundamental ways of solving it are presented: open-loop (feed forward) and closed-loop (feedback) control. The next section takes a closer look at feed-forward processes, which are initiated before a movement starts. They are, therefore, discussed under the heading of Motor Preparation. Subsequently, research on the use of sensory information in motor control is summarized. Closed-loop control involves the exploitation of sensory feedback from an ongoing movement, but sensory information may also be used for other purposes, such as during movement preparation. After the discussion of these fundamental issues, the topic of motor coordination is addressed. The problems that arise when two or more concurrent movements have to be coordinated are discussed in detail with special reference to bimanual coordination. In the final section, we review the empirical evidence for the tremendous flexibility of motor control, which is a prerequisite of human skills in operating tools and machines and in handling objects of various masses. Keywords: movement control; motor control; motor coordination; visuo-motor transformation; bimanual coordination

[1]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[2]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. , 1987, Canadian journal of psychology.

[3]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Bimanual coupling during the specification of isometric forces , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[4]  D. Meyer,et al.  Conditions for a Linear Speed-Accuracy Trade-Off in Aimed Movements , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[5]  M. Latash,et al.  Anticipatory postural adjustments under simple and choice reaction time conditions , 2002, Brain Research.

[6]  L. Thaler,et al.  The control parameters used by the CNS to guide the hand depend on the visuo-motor task: Evidence from visually guided pointing , 2009, Neuroscience.

[7]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[8]  Daniel Cattaert,et al.  Simulating a neural cross-talk model for between-hand interference during bimanual circle drawing , 1999, Biological Cybernetics.

[9]  D Deutsch,et al.  The generation of two isochronous sequences in parallel , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[10]  T. Flash,et al.  The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[11]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects , 1998, Neuropsychologia.

[12]  Auke Jan Ijspeert,et al.  Central pattern generators for locomotion control in animals and robots: A review , 2008, Neural Networks.

[13]  H. Heuer Temporal and spatial characteristics of rapid finger oscillations. , 2006, Motor control.

[14]  D. Rosenbaum The Movement Precuing Technique: Assumptions, Applications, and Extensions , 1983 .

[15]  E A Franz,et al.  Spatial Coupling in the Coordination of Complex Actions , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Why don't we perceive our brain states? , 1992 .

[17]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Involuntary Rotations of a Steering Device Induced by Voluntary Rotations of the Head and Maintained Eccentric Head Positions. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.

[18]  R. Abrams,et al.  Differential use of distance and location information for spatial localization , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  P. Viviani,et al.  The law relating the kinematic and figural aspects of drawing movements. , 1983, Acta psychologica.

[20]  M. Fahle,et al.  Grasping Visual Illusions: No Evidence for a Dissociation Between Perception and Action , 2000, Psychological science.

[21]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Listening to action-related sentences modulates the activity of the motor system: a combined TMS and behavioral study. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[22]  K. Kalveram,et al.  Pattern generating and reflex-like processes controlling aiming movements in the presence of inertia, damping and gravity , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.

[23]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Early stages in a sensorimotor transformation , 1992, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[24]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Optic ataxia revisited: , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  T. Brashers-Krug,et al.  Functional Stages in the Formation of Human Long-Term Motor Memory , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  David N. Lee,et al.  A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.

[27]  Cristina Massen,et al.  What to do and how to do it: action representations in tool use , 2012, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  BRENDAN O. MCGONIGLE,et al.  Long-term retention of single and multistate prismatic adaptation by humans , 1978, Nature.

[29]  Gerald L. Gottlieb,et al.  Fatigue induced changes in phasic muscle activation patterns for fast elbow flexion movements , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[30]  J. M. Hollerbach,et al.  Deducing planning variables from experimental arm trajectories: Pitfalls and possibilities , 1987, Biological Cybernetics.

[31]  R A Abrams,et al.  Optimality in human motor performance: ideal control of rapid aimed movements. , 1988, Psychological review.

[32]  Paolo Cavallari,et al.  Preferential coupling between voluntary movements of ipsilateral limbs , 1982, Neuroscience Letters.

[33]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Functional independence of explicit and implicit motor adjustments , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[34]  John Morton,et al.  Rhythm and dominance , 1981, Cognition.

[35]  R. Ivry,et al.  Moving to Directly Cued Locations Abolishes Spatial Interference During Bimanual Actions , 2001, Psychological science.

[36]  R. Thelwell,et al.  Soccer penalty takers' uniform colour and pre-penalty kick gaze affect the impressions formed of them by opposing goalkeepers , 2008, Journal of sports sciences.

[37]  S. Cardoso de Oliveira,et al.  Visuomotor transformations affect bimanual coupling , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  O. Bock,et al.  Error accumulation and error correction in sequential pointing movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  M. Perenin,et al.  Visual function within the hemianopic field following early cerebral hemidecortication in man—I. Spatial localization , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[40]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes: specification interference as revealed by reaction time , 1997 .

[41]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  Spatial topological constraints in a bimanual task. , 1991, Acta psychologica.

[42]  O. Bock Load compensation in human goal-directed arm movements , 1990, Behavioural Brain Research.

[43]  R Shadmehr,et al.  Spatial Generalization from Learning Dynamics of Reaching Movements , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[44]  Christian Keysers,et al.  The anthropomorphic brain: The mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions , 2007, NeuroImage.

[45]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[46]  R. Plamondon,et al.  Optimal Movement Selection , 1991 .

[47]  Torsten Ingemann Nielsen,et al.  VOLITION: A NEW EXPERIMENTAL APPROACH , 1963 .

[48]  H. Haken,et al.  A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[49]  R J Bootsma,et al.  Accuracy of Perceptual Processes Subserving Different Perception–Action Systems , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[50]  R J Jagacinski,et al.  Tests of parallel versus integrated structure in polyrhythmic tapping. , 1988, Journal of motor behavior.

[51]  Cristina Massen,et al.  Coordinative constraints in bimanual tool use , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movements, actions and tool-use actions: an ideomotor approach to imitation , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[53]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Delayed auditory feedback in synchronization. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[54]  D. A. Winter,et al.  Joit torque and energy patterns in normal gait , 1978, Biological Cybernetics.

[55]  W. Helsen,et al.  A century later: Woodworth's (1899) two-component model of goal-directed aiming. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.

[56]  D J Glencross,et al.  Serial organization and timing in a motor skill. , 1970, Journal of motor behavior.

[57]  E. Bizzi,et al.  Characteristics of motor programs underlying arm movements in monkeys. , 1979, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Loukia D. Loukopoulos,et al.  Planning reaches by evaluating stored postures. , 1995, Psychological review.

[59]  Harsha Sirisena,et al.  Detection of adaptive inverse models in the human motor system , 2000 .

[60]  H N Zelaznik,et al.  The specification of digit and duration during motor programming: a new method of precueing. , 1982, Journal of motor behavior.

[61]  Klaus Roth,et al.  Chapter 11 Investigations on the Basis of the Generalized Motor Programme Hypothesis , 1988 .

[62]  H. Heuer,et al.  Task-dependent mixtures of coordinate systems in visuomotor transformations , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  Hank Heijink,et al.  Different learned coordinate frames for planning trajectories and final positions in reaching. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[64]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[65]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  The dynamics of memory as a consequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body , 2007, Nature Neuroscience.

[66]  C. Bard,et al.  Control of single-joint movements in deafferented patients: evidence for amplitude coding rather than position control , 1996, Experimental Brain Research.

[67]  J. Flanagan,et al.  Coupling of grip force and load force during arm movements with grasped objects , 1993, Neuroscience Letters.

[68]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Three-Dimensional Movement Trajectories in Fitts' Task: Implications for Control , 1987 .

[69]  M. Goodale,et al.  An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action , 2004, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[70]  W. Verwey Effect of Sequence Length on the Execution of Familiar Keying Sequences: Lasting Segmentation and Preparation? , 2003, Journal of motor behavior.

[71]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[72]  Felice L. Bedford,et al.  Constraints on learning new mappings between perceptual dimensions , 1989 .

[73]  H. C. Dijkerman,et al.  Interference of grasping observation during prehension, a behavioural study , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[74]  K. Lashley THE ACCURACY OF MOVEMENT IN THE ABSENCE OF EXCITATION FROM THE MOVING ORGAN , 1917 .

[75]  J. Krakauer,et al.  An Implicit Plan Overrides an Explicit Strategy during Visuomotor Adaptation , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[76]  M. Hallett Volitional control of movement: The physiology of free will , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[77]  P. Haggard,et al.  On the relation between brain potentials and the awareness of voluntary movements , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[78]  A. W. Blöte,et al.  Task effects on young children's performance in manipulating a pencil , 1989 .

[79]  P. Rack,et al.  The effects of length and stimulus rate on tension in the isometric cat soleus muscle , 1969, The Journal of physiology.

[80]  Rieko Osu,et al.  The central nervous system stabilizes unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance , 2001, Nature.

[81]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Generalized motor programs for rapid bimanual tasks: a two-level multiplicative-rate model , 1995, Biological Cybernetics.

[82]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Type of visual feedback during practice influences the precision of the acquired internal model of a complex visuo-motor transformation , 2011, Ergonomics.

[83]  P. Viviani,et al.  Motor strategies in lifting movements: a comparison of adult and child performance. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[84]  Y. Guiard Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: the kinematic chain as a model. , 1987, Journal of motor behavior.

[85]  Lawrence W. Stark,et al.  Sensing and Manipulation Problems in Endoscopic Surgery: Experiment, Analysis, and Observation , 1993, Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments.

[86]  S. Chieffi,et al.  Visual illusion and action , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[87]  J. Tresilian Hitting a moving target: Perception and action in the timing of rapid interceptions , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[88]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[89]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Learning the visuomotor transformation of virtual and real sliding levers: simple approximations of complex transformations , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[90]  Herbert Heuer Blocking in rapid finger tapping: the role of variability in proximodistal coordination. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[91]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Movement control in skilled motor performance. , 1968 .

[92]  Shumin Zhai,et al.  Characterizing computer input with Fitts' law parameters-the information and non-information aspects of pointing , 2004, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[93]  Robert E. Dewar Adaptation to displaced vision: Variations on the “prismatic-shaping” technique , 1971 .

[94]  W. Prinz,et al.  Activation of action rules in action observation. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[95]  W Spijkers,et al.  Temporal coordination of alternative and simultaneous aiming movements of constrained timing structure , 1994, Psychological research.

[96]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual and proprioceptive adaptation to optical displacement of the visual stimulus. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[97]  H. Cruse,et al.  The human arm as a redundant manipulator: The control of path and joint angles , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[98]  Daniel Bullock,et al.  Neural dynamics of planned arm movements: emergent invariants and speed-accuracy properties during trajectory formation , 1988 .

[99]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal-selection and movement-related conflict during bimanual reaching movements. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[100]  B. Libet,et al.  Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. , 1983 .

[101]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[102]  W. Prinz,et al.  Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination , 2001, Nature.

[103]  H. Kornhuber,et al.  Hirnpotentialänderungen bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale , 1965, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[104]  S. Glasauer,et al.  Moving weightless objects , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[105]  D. Meyer,et al.  Models for the speed and accuracy of aimed movements. , 1982, Psychological review.

[106]  Mathias Hegele,et al.  Constraints on visuo-motor adaptation depend on the type of visual feedback during practice , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[107]  J. I. Todor,et al.  EXERTION LEVEL AND THE INTENSITY OF ASSOCIATED MOVEMENTS , 1986, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[108]  W. L. Nelson Physical principles for economies of skilled movements , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[109]  A M Wing,et al.  Brief bimanual force pulses: correlations between the hands in force and time. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[110]  H. Heuer,et al.  Implicit and explicit adjustments to extrinsic visuo-motor transformations and their age-related changes. , 2011, Human movement science.

[111]  R. Welch The effect of experienced limb identity upon adaptation to simulated displacement of the visual field , 1972 .

[112]  S. Riek,et al.  Dual adaptation to two opposing visuomotor rotations when each is associated with different regions of workspace , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[113]  Will Spijkers,et al.  The time course of cross-talk during the simultaneous specification of bimanual movement amplitudes , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[114]  H. Heuer,et al.  The modulation of intermanual interactions during the specification of the directions of bimanual movements , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[115]  Arnaud Badets,et al.  Intention in motor learning through observation , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[116]  D A Rosenbaum,et al.  Production of polyrhythms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[117]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Static and Phasic Cross-Talk Effects in Discrete Bimanual Reversal Movements , 2001, Journal of motor behavior.

[118]  R Plamondon,et al.  Speed/accuracy trade-offs in target-directed movements , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[119]  F. Bedford Constraints on perceptual learning: objects and dimensions , 1995, Cognition.

[120]  Iseult A. M. Beets,et al.  Nonvisual motor learning improves visual motion perception: evidence from violating the two-thirds power law. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[121]  Herbert Heuer Will Spijkers Thomas Kleinsorge Hanno van de Loo Period Duration of Physical and Im aginary Movement Sequences Affects Contralateral Amplitude Modulation , 1998 .

[122]  Roland S. Johansson,et al.  Sensory and Memory Information in the Control of Dexterous Manipulation , 1996 .

[123]  R. Bootsma,et al.  Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. , 1990 .

[124]  O. Bock,et al.  Goal-directed arm movements in absence of visual guidance: evidence for amplitude rather than position control , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[125]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[126]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Preserved Adjustment but Impaired Awareness in a Sensory-Motor Conflict following Prefrontal Lesions , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[127]  C. Ghez,et al.  Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[128]  R. Schmidt A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. , 1975 .

[129]  Karl M. Newell,et al.  The acquisition of coordination: Preliminary analysis of learning to write , 1989 .

[130]  H. Heuer,et al.  The effects of mechanical transparency on adjustment to a complex visuomotor transformation at early and late working age. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[131]  Richard Latto,et al.  Is blindsight an effect of scattered light, spared cortex, and near-threshold vision? , 1983, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[132]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Utilization of Visual Feedback Information during Rapid Pointing Movements , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[133]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[134]  J. Wann,et al.  Does limb proprioception drift? , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[135]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Does a tool eliminate spatial compatibility effects? , 2008 .

[136]  L. Cohen,et al.  Synchronous Bimanual Movements Performed by Homologous and Non-Homologous Muscles , 1971, Perceptual and motor skills.

[137]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Behavioral Principles of Interlimb Coordination , 2004 .

[138]  K. Davids,et al.  Timing a one-handed catch , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[139]  Stephen Monsell,et al.  Programming of Complex Sequences: Evidence from the Timing of Rapid Speech and Other Productions , 1986 .

[140]  Sarah H. Creem,et al.  Defining the cortical visual systems: "what", "where", and "how". , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[141]  Marco Favilla,et al.  Parallel direction and extent specification of planar reaching arm movements in humans , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[142]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[143]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[144]  Cristina Massen Observing human interaction with physical devices , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[146]  G. Stratton Some preliminary experiments on vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1896 .

[147]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Invariant relative timing in motor-program theory , 1991 .

[148]  S. Swinnen,et al.  Interlimb coordination: Learning and transfer under different feedback conditions , 1997 .

[149]  J. C. Ruitenbeek Invariants in loaded goal directed movements , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[150]  H. Heuer,et al.  Estimates of Time to Contact Based on Changing Size and Changing Target Vergence , 1993, Perception.

[151]  Les G. Carlton,et al.  Visual Information: The Control of Aiming Movements , 1981 .

[152]  Timothy D. Lee,et al.  Effects of task instructions and oscillation frequency on bimanual coordination , 1996, Psychological research.

[153]  Lorraine G. Kisselburgh,et al.  Rapid visual feedback processing in single-aiming movements. , 1983, Journal of motor behavior.

[154]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[155]  C. Ghez,et al.  Trajectory control in targeted force impulses , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[156]  J. Kelso,et al.  Exploring a vibratory systems analysis of human movement production. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[157]  Geoffrey P Bingham,et al.  Calibrating reach distance to visual targets. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[158]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[159]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  The basal ganglia network mediates the planning of movement amplitude , 2004, The European journal of neuroscience.

[160]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Nonlinear visuomotor transformations: Locus and modularity , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[161]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[162]  M A Goodale,et al.  The effects of instructions to subjects on the programming of visually directed reaching movements. , 1989, Journal of motor behavior.

[163]  Rieko Osu,et al.  Short- and long-term changes in joint co-contraction associated with motor learning as revealed from surface EMG. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[164]  M. Peters,et al.  Attentional Asymmetries during Concurrent Bimanual Performance , 1981 .

[165]  H. Heuer Simultaneous Specification of Amplitudes and Directions of Bimanual Reversal Movements , 2006, Journal of motor behavior.

[166]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Temporal compatibility in dual motor tasks II: Simultaneous articulation and hand movements , 1981, Memory & cognition.

[167]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[168]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  Doing two things at once: The role of temporal compatibility , 1979 .

[169]  A. Mack,et al.  The dissociation of position and extent in Müller-Lyer figures , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[170]  J. F. Schouten,et al.  Reaction time and accuracy. , 1967, Acta psychologica.

[171]  R. Ellis,et al.  Action priming by briefly presented objects. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[172]  M. Posner,et al.  Processing of visual feedback in rapid movements. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[173]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Short-Term Memory for Reaching to Visual Targets: Psychophysical Evidence for Body-Centered Reference Frames , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[174]  D. Sherwood Distance and location assimilation effects in rapid bimanual movement. , 1991, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[175]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Utilization of Sensory Information for Motor Control , 1990 .

[176]  C. Bard,et al.  The role of proprioceptive information for the production of isometric forces and for handwriting tasks. , 1993, Acta psychologica.

[177]  H. Heuer,et al.  The configuration and relaxation of motor task sets , 2007, Psychological research.

[178]  Ping Zhou,et al.  Factors governing the form of the relation between muscle force and the EMG: a simulation study. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[179]  Cristina Massen,et al.  Bimanual interference with compatible and incompatible tool transformations. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[180]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Motor facilitation following action observation: A behavioural study in prehensile action , 2003, Brain and Cognition.

[181]  W. Spijkers,et al.  Partial visual feedback and spatial end-point accuracy of discrete aiming movements. , 1994, Journal of motor behavior.

[182]  J. Cooke,et al.  Effects of gravitational forces on single joint arm movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[183]  D Goodman,et al.  On the coordination of two-handed movements. , 1979, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[184]  Albert Derwort,et al.  Untersuchungen über den Zeitablauf figurierter Bewegungen beim Menschen , 1938, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[185]  C. MacKenzie,et al.  Bimanual Movement Control: Information processing and Interaction Effects , 1984 .

[186]  W Schiff,et al.  Information Used in Judging Impending Collision , 1979, Perception.

[187]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Early stages of load compensation in human aimed arm movements , 1993, Behavioural Brain Research.

[188]  J R Tresilian,et al.  Analysis of recent empirical challenges to an account of interceptive timing , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.

[189]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Intermanual interactions in discrete and periodic bimanual movements with same and different amplitudes , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[190]  Will Spijkers,et al.  Structural Constraints on the Performance of Symmetrical Bimanual Movements with Different Amplitudes , 1995 .

[191]  Miya K. Rand,et al.  Quantitative model of transport-aperture coordination during reach-to-grasp movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[193]  Dietrich Manzey,et al.  Changed visuomotor transformations during and after prolonged microgravity , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[194]  J. Kelso,et al.  A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned patterns of movement coordination , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.

[195]  R. Schmidt,et al.  Transfer of learning among motor patterns with different relative timing. , 1988 .

[196]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  Control of the dominant and nondominant hand: exploitation and taming of nonmuscular forces , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[197]  R W Pew,et al.  Two quantitative measures of skill development. , 1971, Journal of experimental psychology.

[198]  B Gillam,et al.  Size and position are incongruous: Measurements on the Müller-Lyer figure , 1985, Perception & psychophysics.

[199]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.

[200]  I. Cockerill,et al.  Eyes on hand-eyes on target? , 1974, Journal of motor behavior.

[201]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[202]  Alwin Luttmann,et al.  The effects of muscle fatigue on rapid finger oscillations , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[203]  W. Prinz,et al.  Programming tool-use actions. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[204]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[205]  E. Todorov Optimality principles in sensorimotor control , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[206]  Slobodan Jaric,et al.  Organization of Drinking: Postural Characteristics of Arm-Head Coordination , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[207]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Control of multimovement coordination: sensorimotor mechanisms in speech motor programming. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[208]  G Aschersleben,et al.  Synchronizing actions with events: The role of sensory information , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[209]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  Tapping with peripheral nerve block , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[210]  Elliot Saltzman,et al.  Skilled actions: a task-dynamic approach. , 1987, Psychological review.

[211]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Visuo-motor coordination and internal models for object interception , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.

[212]  James Gordon,et al.  Accuracy of planar reaching movements , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[213]  D. Gentner Timing of Skilled Motor Performance: Tests of the Proportional Duration Model. , 1987 .

[214]  L. Selen,et al.  Impedance Control Reduces Instability That Arises from Motor Noise , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[215]  W. Prinz,et al.  Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[216]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Spatial Compatibility Effects With Tool Use , 2007, Hum. Factors.

[217]  N. Teasdale,et al.  Pointing to a target from an upright standing position: anticipatory postural adjustments are modulated by the size of the target in humans , 2004, Neuroscience Letters.

[218]  Lore Thaler,et al.  Evidence from visuomotor adaptation for two partially independent visuomotor systems. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[219]  I. Johnsrude,et al.  Somatotopic Representation of Action Words in Human Motor and Premotor Cortex , 2004, Neuron.

[220]  A. Gallagher,et al.  Experienced laparoscopic surgeons are automated to the "fulcrum effect": an ergonomic demonstration. , 1999, Endoscopy.

[221]  Raymond H. Cuijpers,et al.  Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[222]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons , 2002, Science.

[223]  Sascha E. Engelbrecht,et al.  Minimum Principles in Motor Control. , 2001, Journal of mathematical psychology.

[224]  P. Wolff,et al.  A functional analysis of human mirror movements. , 1997, Journal of motor behavior.

[225]  P. Cavallari,et al.  Differential control of in-phase and anti-phase coupling of rhythmic movements of ipsilateral hand and foot , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[226]  L Knuf,et al.  An analysis of ideomotor action. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[227]  J. Crawford,et al.  Gaze-Centered Remapping of Remembered Visual Space in an Open-Loop Pointing Task , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[228]  M. Kawato,et al.  Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement , 1989, Biological Cybernetics.

[229]  Jeroen B J Smeets,et al.  Relative damping improves linear mass-spring models of goal-directed movements. , 2002, Human movement science.

[230]  Y. Rossetti,et al.  Two waves of a long-lasting aftereffect of prism adaptation measured over 7 days , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[231]  S. P. Swinnen,et al.  The identification of coordination constraints across planes of motion , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[232]  P. Fitts The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[233]  J. Summers,et al.  The Dynamics of Bimanual Circle Drawing , 1997, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[234]  M. Goodale,et al.  The objects of action and perception , 1998, Cognition.

[235]  H. Zelaznik,et al.  Motor-output variability: a theory for the accuracy of rapid motor acts. , 1979, Psychological review.

[236]  W. Prinz,et al.  Instructions for motor learning: differential effects of internal versus external focus of attention. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.

[237]  Geoffrey Bird,et al.  Effector-dependent learning by observation of a finger movement sequence. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[238]  Alan Cowey,et al.  The neurobiology of blindsight , 1991, Trends in Neurosciences.

[239]  G. Schott,et al.  Congenital mirror movements. , 1981, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[240]  A. Hamilton,et al.  Interference effect of observed human movement on action is due to velocity profile of biological motion , 2007, Social neuroscience.

[241]  E Brenner,et al.  Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[242]  D. Elliott,et al.  The Influence of Premovement Visual Information on Manual Aiming , 1987, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[243]  Marian Gunkel Über relative Koordination bei willkürlichen menschlichen Gliedbewegungen , 1962, Pflüger's Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere.

[244]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[245]  W. Verwey,et al.  The effect of continuous, nonlinearly transformed visual feedback on rapid aiming movements , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[246]  Nicola Bruno,et al.  When does action resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[247]  Herbert Heuer,et al.  The influence of movement cues on intermanual interactions , 2006, Psychological research.

[248]  Richard A. Schmidt,et al.  The 1984 C. H. McCloy Research Lecture: The Search for Invariance in Skilled Movement Behavior , 1985 .