Deriving Numerosity and Shape from Identical Visual Displays

We presented identical displays of three to five dots in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiment with normal volunteers. Two distinct directed attention tasks were performed on these displays: In one condition, subjects assessed the numerosity of the display; in the other condition, they assessed the shape of the display. Decisions based on numerosity activated differentially striate and extrastriate visual processing areas as well as left inferior frontal cortex. Decisions based on shape derived from arrangement activated differentially temporoparietal cortex bilaterally, medial posterior cingulate cortex, and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These divergent neural activations in response to identical stimuli suggest that attentional mechanisms are deployed in very different ways in rapid enumeration of visual objects and in linking spatially discrete elements to one form.

[1]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The physiological basis of attentional modulation in extrastriate visual areas , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[2]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Retinotopic Maps in Human Prestriate Visual Cortex: The Demarcation of Areas V2 and V3 , 1995, NeuroImage.

[3]  B. Julesz,et al.  Perceptual sensitivity maps within globally defined visual shapes , 1994, Nature.

[4]  C D Frith,et al.  Space-based and object-based visual attention: shared and specific neural domains. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  A. Treisman Features and Objects: The Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[6]  D. Somers,et al.  Functional MRI reveals spatially specific attentional modulation in human primary visual cortex. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  James T. Enns,et al.  Clusters Precede Shapes in Perceptual Organization , 1997 .

[8]  J W Belliveau,et al.  Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Science.

[9]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .

[10]  R Shapley,et al.  Illusory contours activate specific regions in human visual cortex: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. , 1995, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[12]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[13]  S J Luck,et al.  Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Where in the brain does visual attention select the forest and the trees? , 1996, Nature.

[15]  R. Turner,et al.  Characterizing Evoked Hemodynamics with fMRI , 1995, NeuroImage.

[16]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  A Locus in Human Extrastriate Cortex for Visual Shape Analysis , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Dissociable mechanisms of subitizing and counting: Neuropsychological evidence from simultanagnosic patients. , 1994 .

[18]  E. Maguire,et al.  Knowing Where Things Are: Parahippocampal Involvement in Encoding Object Locations in Virtual Large-Scale Space , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The neural regions sustaining object recognition and naming , 1996, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[21]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Top-down modulation of early sensory cortex. , 1997 .

[22]  S. Zeki,et al.  Brain Activity Related to the Perception of Illusory Contours , 1996, NeuroImage.

[23]  K. Zilles,et al.  Line bisection judgments implicate right parietal cortex and cerebellum as assessed by fMRI , 2000, Neurology.

[24]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Dissociating working memory from task difficulty in human prefrontal cortex , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Neural Evidence Linking Visual Object Enumeration and Attention , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[26]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[27]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[28]  Daniel L. Schacter,et al.  Brain regions associated with retrieval of structurally coherent visual information , 1995, Nature.

[29]  R. Desimone Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[30]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal modulation of visual processing in humans , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[31]  L. Robertson,et al.  Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organization , 1991, Cognitive Psychology.

[32]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[33]  Masamichi Sakagami,et al.  The hierarchical organization of decision making in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1999, Neuroscience Research.

[34]  E. Warrington,et al.  Two Categorical Stages of Object Recognition , 1978, Perception.

[35]  I. Kovács Gestalten of today: early processing of visual contours and surfaces , 1996, Behavioural Brain Research.

[36]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[37]  M M Mesulam,et al.  Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory , 1990, Annals of neurology.

[38]  C D Frith,et al.  Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli. , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[39]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[40]  W Singer,et al.  Consciousness and the structure of neuronal representations. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[41]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[42]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[43]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[44]  K. Amunts,et al.  Brodmann's Areas 17 and 18 Brought into Stereotaxic Space—Where and How Variable? , 2000, NeuroImage.

[45]  M. Corbetta,et al.  A PET study of visuospatial attention , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[46]  G. Winocur,et al.  Dissociation of pathways for object and spatial vision: a PET study in humans , 1995, Neuroreport.