Bright illusions reduce the eye's pupil

We recorded by use of an infrared eye-tracker the pupil diameters of participants while they observed visual illusions of lightness or brightness. Four original illusions {based on Gaetano Kanisza's [Kanizsa G (1976) Subjective contours. Sci Am 234:48–52] and Akiyoshi Kitaoka's [Kitaoka A. (2005) Trick Eyes (Barnes & Noble, New Providence, NJ).] examples} were manipulated to obtain control conditions in which the perceived illusory luminance was either eliminated or reduced. All stimuli were equiluminant so that constrictions in pupillary size could not be ascribed to changes in light energy. We found that the pupillary diameter rapidly varied according to perceived brightness and lightness strength. Differences in local contrast information could be ruled out as an explanation because, in a second experiment, the observers maintained eye fixation in the center of the display; thus, differential stimulation of the fovea by local contrast changes could not be responsible for the pupillary differences. Hence, the most parsimonious explanation for the present findings is that pupillary responses to ambient light reflect the perceived brightness or lightness of the scene and not simply the amount of physical light energy entering the eye. Thus, the pupillary physiological response reflects the subjective perception of light and supports the idea that the brain's visual circuitry is shaped by visual experience with images and their possible sources.

[1]  D Kahneman,et al.  Pupil Diameter and Load on Memory , 1966, Science.

[2]  W Lewin,et al.  Cortical blindness and the functions of the non-geniculate fibres of the optic tracts. , 1969, Journal of neurology, neurosurgery, and psychiatry.

[3]  D. Kahneman Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[4]  G. Kanizsa Subjective contours. , 1976, Scientific American.

[5]  D. van Norren,et al.  Visual acuity measured with pupil responses to checkerboard stimuli. , 1980, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[6]  J. Beatty Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. , 1982, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  R. Kardon,et al.  Automated pupil perimetry. Pupil field mapping in patients and normal subjects. , 1991, Ophthalmology.

[8]  D. Perrett,et al.  Time course of neural responses discriminating different views of the face and head. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  L. Spillmann,et al.  Phenomena of Illusory Form: Can We Bridge the Gap between Levels of Explanation? , 1995, Perception.

[10]  Denis Fize,et al.  Speed of processing in the human visual system , 1996, Nature.

[11]  R. Vogels Categorization of complex visual images by rhesus monkeys. Part 2: single‐cell study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.

[12]  L Weiskrantz,et al.  Differential pupillary constriction and awareness in the absence of striate cortex. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  I. E. Loewenfeld,et al.  The Pupil: Anatomy, Physiology, and Clinical Applications , 1999 .

[14]  Shinsuke Shimojo,et al.  Pupillary response to chromatic flicker , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[15]  Sean P. MacEvoy,et al.  Lightness constancy in primary visual cortex , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[16]  Sean P. MacEvoy,et al.  Perception of Brightness and Brightness Illusions in the Macaque Monkey , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Paul D. Gamlin,et al.  Characteristics of the pupillary light reflex in the alert rhesus monkey. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  D. Perrett,et al.  Visual neurones responsive to faces in the monkey temporal cortex , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  Fuchuan Sun,et al.  Pupillary response induced by stereoscopic stimuli , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. , 2005, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  N. Newman The Visual Neurosciences , 2005 .

[22]  C. Hung,et al.  Cortical processing of a brightness illusion. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  Bruno Laeng,et al.  Pupillometry of Grapheme-Color Synaesthesia , 2006, Cortex.

[24]  J. Cacioppo,et al.  Handbook Of Psychophysiology , 2019 .

[25]  Ryota Kanai,et al.  Perceiving the Present and a Systematization of Illusions , 2008, Cogn. Sci..

[26]  C. Koch,et al.  Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[27]  Richard L Gregory,et al.  Seeing Through Illusions , 2009 .

[28]  Michele Miozzo,et al.  Pupillary Stroop effects , 2010, Cognitive Processing.

[29]  D. Purves,et al.  Why we see what we do redux : a wholly empirical theory of vision , 2011 .

[30]  G. Altmann Language can mediate eye movement control within 100 milliseconds, regardless of whether there is anything to move the eyes to , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[31]  Dale Purves,et al.  Understanding vision in wholly empirical terms , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  S. Sirois,et al.  Pupillometry , 2012, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.