Cue integration outside central fixation: a study of grasping in depth.

We assessed the usefulness of stereopsis across the visual field by quantifying how retinal eccentricity and distance from the horopter affect humans' relative dependence on monocular and binocular cues about 3D orientation. The reliabilities of monocular and binocular cues both decline with eccentricity, but the reliability of binocular information decreases more rapidly. Binocular cue reliability also declines with increasing distance from the horopter, whereas the reliability of monocular cues is virtually unaffected. We measured how subjects integrated these cues to orient their hands when grasping oriented discs at different eccentricities and distances from the horopter. Subjects relied increasingly less on binocular disparity as targets' retinal eccentricity and distance from the horopter increased. The measured cue influences were consistent with what would be predicted from the relative cue reliabilities at the various target locations. Our results showed that relative reliability affects how cues influence motor control and that stereopsis is of limited use in the periphery and away from the horopter because monocular cues are more reliable in these regions.

[1]  Martin S Banks,et al.  The surface of the empirical horopter. , 2008, Journal of vision.

[2]  D. Knill Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[3]  D. R. Hampton,et al.  The Extent of Panum's Area and the Human Cortical Magnification Factor , 1983, Perception.

[4]  James M. Hillis,et al.  Slant from texture and disparity cues: optimal cue combination. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[5]  K. N. Ogle On the limits of stereoscopic vision. , 1952, Journal of experimental psychology.

[6]  C. Blakemore The range and scope of binocular depth discrimination in man , 1970, The Journal of physiology.

[7]  M F Armaly,et al.  The size and location of the normal blind spot. , 1969, Archives of ophthalmology.

[8]  A. Parker,et al.  Efficiency of stereopsis in random-dot stereograms. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[9]  M. Hayhoe,et al.  In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? , 2001, Vision Research.

[10]  A. Parker,et al.  Stereoacuity thresholds in the presence of a reference surface , 2001, Vision Research.

[11]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Forward modeling allows feedback control for fast reaching movements , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  Heinrich H. Bülthoff,et al.  Touch can change visual slant perception , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  A. Cowey,et al.  Human cortical magnification factor and its relation to visual acuity , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[14]  D. Burr,et al.  The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.

[15]  D. Knill Learning Bayesian priors for depth perception. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[16]  L K Cormack,et al.  Element density and the efficiency of binocular matching. , 1997, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[17]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[18]  L. Thibos,et al.  Retinal limits to the detection and resolution of gratings. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[19]  R. Harwerth,et al.  Stereopsis, spatial frequency and retinal eccentricity , 1995, Vision Research.

[20]  G. Westheimer,et al.  Effects of practice and the separation of test targets on foveal and peripheral stereoacuity , 1983, Vision Research.

[21]  J. Saunders,et al.  Do humans optimally integrate stereo and texture information for judgments of surface slant? , 2003, Vision Research.

[22]  M. Landy,et al.  Why Is Spatial Stereoresolution So Low? , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[23]  L. Jakobson,et al.  Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Aarlenne Z. Khan,et al.  Coordinating one hand with two eyes: optimizing for field of view in a pointing task , 2003, Vision Research.

[25]  Hal S. Greenwald,et al.  A comparison of visuomotor cue integration strategies for object placement and prehension , 2009, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  David C. Knill,et al.  Integrating visual cues for motor control: A matter of time , 2005, Vision Research.