Causal evidence for frontal involvement in memory target maintenance by posterior brain areas during distracter interference of visual working memory

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is recruited during visual working memory (WM) when relevant information must be maintained in the presence of distracting information. The mechanism by which DLPFC might ensure successful maintenance of the contents of WM is, however, unclear; it might enhance neural maintenance of memory targets or suppress processing of distracters. To adjudicate between these possibilities, we applied time-locked transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) during functional MRI, an approach that permits causal assessment of a stimulated brain region's influence on connected brain regions, and evaluated how this influence may change under different task conditions. Participants performed a visual WM task requiring retention of visual stimuli (faces or houses) across a delay during which visual distracters could be present or absent. When distracters were present, they were always from the opposite stimulus category, so that targets and distracters were represented in distinct posterior cortical areas. We then measured whether DLPFC-TMS, administered in the delay at the time point when distracters could appear, would modulate posterior regions representing memory targets or distracters. We found that DLPFC-TMS influenced posterior areas only when distracters were present and, critically, that this influence consisted of increased activity in regions representing the current memory targets. DLPFC-TMS did not affect regions representing current distracters. These results provide a new line of causal evidence for a top-down DLPFC-based control mechanism that promotes successful maintenance of relevant information in WM in the presence of distraction.

[1]  Earl K. Miller,et al.  Selective representation of relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[2]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Category-specific modulation of inferior temporal activity during working memory encoding and maintenance. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[3]  Hartwig R. Siebner,et al.  BOLD MRI responses to repetitive TMS over human dorsal premotor cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[4]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Distinct causal influences of parietal versus frontal areas on human visual cortex: evidence from concurrent TMS-fMRI. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  J. Duncan An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex , 2001 .

[6]  C. Curtis,et al.  Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  R. Malmo INTERFERENCE FACTORS IN DELAYED RESPONSE IN MONKEYS AFTER REMOVAL OF FRONTAL LOBES , 1942 .

[8]  M. D’Esposito,et al.  Dissecting Contributions of Prefrontal Cortex and Fusiform Face Area to Face Working Memory , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  B. Postle,et al.  Functional neuroanatomical double dissociation of mnemonic and executive control processes contributing to working memory performance. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  Sven Bestmann,et al.  Studying the Role of Human Parietal Cortex in Visuospatial Attention with Concurrent TMS–fMRI , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  John Duncan,et al.  Selective Tuning of the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Response during Simple Target Detection Dissociates Human Frontoparietal Subregions , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Passingham,et al.  Active maintenance in prefrontal area 46 creates distractor-resistant memory , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  R. Knight,et al.  Contribution of Human Prefrontal Cortex to Delay Performance , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Ethan R. Buch,et al.  Distributed and causal influence of frontal operculum in task control , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[16]  John Duncan,et al.  The Target Selective Neural Response — Similarity, Ambiguity, and Learning Effects , 2008, PloS one.

[17]  P. Haggard,et al.  Dorsal premotor cortex exerts state-dependent causal influences on activity in contralateral primary motor and dorsal premotor cortex. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[18]  Bradley R. Postle,et al.  Delay-period Activity in the Prefrontal Cortex: One Function Is Sensory Gating , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[19]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Domain specificity in visual cortex. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[20]  Joseph B. Sala,et al.  Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[21]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  Theodore P. Zanto,et al.  Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.

[23]  B. Postle Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain , 2006, Neuroscience.

[24]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  The Neural Bases of the Short-Term Storage of Verbal Information Are Anatomically Variable across Individuals , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Anna C Nobre,et al.  Subsecond Changes in Top–Down Control Exerted by Human Medial Frontal Cortex during Conflict and Action Selection: A Combined Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation–Electroencephalography Study , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Mechanisms of working memory disruption by external interference. , 2010, Cerebral cortex.

[27]  A. Owen The Functional Organization of Working Memory Processes Within Human Lateral Frontal Cortex: The Contribution of Functional Neuroimaging , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.

[28]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[29]  Philip A. Kragel,et al.  Regional brain differences in the effect of distraction during the delay interval of a working memory task , 2007, Brain Research.

[30]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Concurrent TMS-fMRI and Psychophysics Reveal Frontal Influences on Human Retinotopic Visual Cortex , 2006, Current Biology.

[31]  Sven Bestmann,et al.  Concurrent brain-stimulation and neuroimaging for studies of cognition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  R. Deichmann,et al.  Image Artifacts in Concurrent Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) and fMRI Caused by Leakage Currents: Modeling and Compensation , 2009, Journal of magnetic resonance imaging : JMRI.

[33]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Optimizing Functional Accuracy of TMS in Cognitive Studies: A Comparison of Methods , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Katsuyuki Sakai,et al.  Task-specific signal transmission from prefrontal cortex in visual selective attention , 2009, Nature Neuroscience.

[35]  T. Klingberg,et al.  Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  Nikolaus Weiskopf,et al.  Interhemispheric Effect of Parietal TMS on Somatosensory Response Confirmed Directly with Concurrent TMS–fMRI , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[37]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Maro G. Machizawa,et al.  Neural measures reveal individual differences in controlling access to working memory , 2005, Nature.

[39]  Giulio Tononi,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Dissociates Working Memory Manipulation from Retention Functions in the Prefrontal, but not Posterior Parietal, Cortex , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[40]  J. Duncan,et al.  Filtering of neural signals by focused attention in the monkey prefrontal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[41]  E. Miller,et al.  An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. , 2001, Annual review of neuroscience.

[42]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.