The influence of visual landscape on the free flight behavior of the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster.

To study the visual cues that control steering behavior in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, we reconstructed three-dimensional trajectories from images taken by stereo infrared video cameras during free flight within structured visual landscapes. Flies move through their environment using a series of straight flight segments separated by rapid turns, termed saccades, during which the fly alters course by approximately 90 degrees in less than 100 ms. Altering the amount of background visual contrast caused significant changes in the fly's translational velocity and saccade frequency. Between saccades, asymmetries in the estimates of optic flow induce gradual turns away from the side experiencing a greater motion stimulus, a behavior opposite to that predicted by a flight control model based upon optomotor equilibrium. To determine which features of visual motion trigger saccades, we reconstructed the visual environment from the fly's perspective for each position in the flight trajectory. From these reconstructions, we modeled the fly's estimation of optic flow on the basis of a two-dimensional array of Hassenstein-Reichardt elementary motion detectors and, through spatial summation, the large-field motion stimuli experienced by the fly during the course of its flight. Event-triggered averages of the large-field motion preceding each saccade suggest that image expansion is the signal that triggers each saccade. The asymmetry in output of the local motion detector array prior to each saccade influences the direction (left versus right) but not the magnitude of the rapid turn. Once initiated, visual feedback does not appear to influence saccade kinematics further. The total expansion experienced before a saccade was similar for flight within both uniform and visually textured backgrounds. In summary, our data suggest that complex behavioral patterns seen during free flight emerge from interactions between the flight control system and the visual environment.

[1]  S. Laughlin,et al.  Spatio-temporal properties of motion detectors matched to low image velocities in hovering insects , 1997, Vision Research.

[2]  R. Wolf,et al.  Reafferent control of optomotor yaw torque inDrosophila melanogaster , 1988, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[3]  Martin Egelhaaf,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Course Control in Insects , 1989 .

[4]  Karl Geokg Götz,et al.  Optomotorische Untersuchung des visuellen systems einiger Augenmutanten der Fruchtfliege Drosophila , 1964, Kybernetik.

[5]  K E Weber,et al.  Aerial performance of Drosophila melanogaster from populations selected for upwind flight ability. , 1997, The Journal of experimental biology.

[6]  R. Wolf,et al.  On the fine structure of yaw torque in visual flight orientation ofDrosophila melanogaster , 2004, Journal of comparative physiology.

[7]  M. Egelhaaf,et al.  Optomotor course control in flies with largely asymmetric visual input , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[8]  R. Hengstenberg,et al.  The halteres of the blowfly Calliphora , 1994, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[9]  Robert A. Harris,et al.  Adaptation and the temporal delay filter of fly motion detectors , 1999, Vision Research.

[10]  T. S. Collett,et al.  Some operating rules for the optomotor system of a hoverfly during voluntary flight , 1980, Journal of comparative physiology.

[11]  Jochen Zeil,et al.  The territorial flight of male houseflies (Fannia canicularis L.) , 1986, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[12]  C. T. David Height control by free‐flying Drosophila , 1979 .

[13]  A. Borst,et al.  Transient and steady-state response properties of movement detectors. , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[14]  Gilbert,et al.  Resistance reflex that maintains upright head posture in the flesh fly neobellieria bullata (Sarcophagidae) , 1998, The Journal of experimental biology.

[15]  H. Wagner Flow-field variables trigger landing in flies , 1982, Nature.

[16]  G. Laurent,et al.  Computation of Object Approach by a Wide-Field, Motion-Sensitive Neuron , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Hateren,et al.  Blowfly flight and optic flow. I. Thorax kinematics and flight dynamics , 1999, The Journal of experimental biology.

[18]  W P Chan,et al.  Visual input to the efferent control system of a fly's "gyroscope". , 1998, Science.

[19]  Peter Diehl,et al.  Radiotelemetric monitoring of heart-rate responses to song playback in blackbirds (Turdus merula) , 2004, Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology.

[20]  M. Egelhaaf,et al.  Neuronal representation of optic flow experienced by unilaterally blinded flies on their mean walking trajectories , 2000, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[21]  Alexander Borst,et al.  Principles of visual motion detection , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[22]  M V Srinivasan,et al.  How insects infer range from visual motion. , 1993, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[23]  R. Hardie Functional Organization of the Fly Retina , 1985 .

[24]  R. Wolf,et al.  Visual control of straight flight in Drosophila melanogaster , 1990, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[25]  A. Borst,et al.  Dendritic integration and its role in computing image velocity. , 1998, Science.

[26]  A. Borst Time course of the Houseflies' landing response , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  C. David The relationship between body angle and flight speed in free‐flying Drosophila , 1978 .

[28]  A. G. Greenhill Kinematics and Dynamics , 1888, Nature.

[29]  D. Sandeman,et al.  Angular acceleration, compensatory head movements and the halteres of flies (Lucilia serricata) , 1980, Journal of comparative physiology.

[30]  M. Land Motion and vision: why animals move their eyes , 1999, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[31]  A. Borst How Do Flies Land?From behavior to neuronal circuits , 1990 .

[32]  P. Simmons,et al.  Orthopteran DCMD neuron: a reevaluation of responses to moving objects. I. Selective responses to approaching objects. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  N. Strausfeld,et al.  Organization and significance of neurons that detect change of visual depth in the hawk moth Manduca sexta , 2000, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[34]  H. Nalbach,et al.  Visual stabilization in arthropods. , 1993, Reviews of oculomotor research.

[35]  Mandyam V. Srinivasan,et al.  Motion detection in insect orientation and navigation , 1999, Vision Research.

[36]  Martin Egelhaaf,et al.  Visual course control in flies relies on neuronal computation of object and background motion , 1988, Trends in Neurosciences.

[37]  C. David,et al.  The dynamics of height stabilization in Drosophila , 1984 .

[38]  Werner Reichardt,et al.  Visually induced height orientation of the fly Musca domestica , 1975, Biological Cybernetics.

[39]  Holger G. Krapp,et al.  Wide-field, motion-sensitive neurons and matched filters for optic flow fields , 2000, Biological Cybernetics.

[40]  Karl Georg Götz,et al.  Flight control in Drosophila by visual perception of motion , 1968, Kybernetik.

[41]  H. Wagner Flight Performance and Visual Control of Flight of the Free-Flying Housefly (Musca Domestica L.) I. Organization of the Flight Motor , 1986 .

[42]  M. Dickinson,et al.  Haltere-mediated equilibrium reflexes of the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. , 1999, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[43]  R. Hengstenberg,et al.  Estimation of self-motion by optic flow processing in single visual interneurons , 1996, Nature.

[44]  A. Borst,et al.  What kind of movement detector is triggering the landing response of the housefly? , 1986, Biological Cybernetics.

[45]  M. Srinivasan,et al.  Range perception through apparent image speed in freely flying honeybees , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.

[46]  C. Wehrhahn,et al.  Neural circuits mediating visual flight control in flies. II. Separation of two control systems by microsurgical brain lesions , 1990, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[47]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Flight control during ‘free yaw turns’ inDrosophila melanogaster , 1988, Journal of Comparative Physiology A.

[48]  M. Heisenberg,et al.  Vision in Drosophila , 1984 .

[49]  Hateren,et al.  Blowfly flight and optic flow. II. Head movements during flight , 1999, The Journal of experimental biology.

[50]  W Reichardt,et al.  Visual control of orientation behaviour in the fly: Part I. A quantitative analysis , 1976, Quarterly Reviews of Biophysics.

[51]  R. Wolf,et al.  On the fine structure of yaw torque in visual flight orientation ofDrosophila melanogaster , 1979, Journal of comparative physiology.

[52]  G. Laurent,et al.  Invariance of Angular Threshold Computation in a Wide-Field Looming-Sensitive Neuron , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  Erich Buchner,et al.  Elementary detectors for vertical movement in the visual system of Drosophila , 1978, Biological Cybernetics.