Cognitive event-related potential components during continuous recognition memory for pictures.

Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 28 young adult subjects during a continuous recognition memory paradigm, with pictures as stimuli. Subjects were required to determine on each trial if the picture was "new" (never before presented) or "old" (seen previously). To assess differences between primary and secondary memory, old items were presented after lags of 2, 8, and 32 intervening pictures (equiprobable) following their first presentation. The results suggested that a negativity (Cz maximum) at 300 ms was the most likely candidate for the brain event reflecting the retrieval of the item from memory. Old/new effects were modulated by two types of activity, both of which were larger in the ERPs elicited by new items. The earlier of these, possibly similar to the N400, had a duration from about 250-600 ms, began with the N300 deflection, and lasted until "P300" began to decrement. The other was positive, resembled "positive slow wave," onset as P300 began to decrement, and lasted until the end of the recording epoch. There were no consistent effects of item lag on the behavioral data or on any of the ERP components, suggesting that for pictorial stimuli, the distinction between the two types of memory store, primary (i.e., immediate memory) and secondary (newly learned information), may not be relevant. In consonance with the lack of lag effects, it was suggested that the lack of a robust subsequent memory effect on the ERP waveform could have been due to the use of pictures, which may have required less elaborative processing in order to be encoded at input.

[1]  M. Rugg,et al.  The effects of task on the modulation of event-related potentials by word repetition. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[2]  D. A. Walsh,et al.  Contemporary issues and new directions in adult development of learning and memory. , 1980 .

[3]  C. C. Wood,et al.  The ɛ-Adjustment Procedure for Repeated-Measures Analyses of Variance , 1976 .

[4]  A. Kaszniak,et al.  Primary memory and secondary memory in dementia of the Alzheimer type. , 1983, Journal of clinical neuropsychology.

[5]  Michael D. Rugg,et al.  Dissociation of Semantic Priming, Word and Non-Word Repetition Effects by Event-Related Potentials , 1987 .

[6]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Scalp distributions of event-related potentials: an ambiguity associated with analysis of variance models. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[7]  C. C. Wood,et al.  ERPs predictive of subsequent recall and recognition performance , 1988, Biological Psychology.

[8]  J. Rohrbaugh,et al.  Electrocortical signs of levels of processing: perceptual analysis and recognition memory. , 1980, Psychophysiology.

[9]  R. C. Oldfield The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.

[10]  M. Potter,et al.  Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures. , 1969, Journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. , 1980, Science.

[12]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  P300 and long-term memory: latency predicts recognition performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[13]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[14]  M. Kutas,et al.  Reading between the lines: Event-related brain potentials during natural sentence processing , 1980, Brain and Language.

[15]  E. Donchin,et al.  “P300” and memory: Individual differences in the von Restorff effect , 1984, Cognitive Psychology.

[16]  V. S. Reed,et al.  Pictorial superiority effect. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human learning and memory.

[17]  E. Donchin,et al.  P300 and recall in an incidental memory paradigm. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[18]  M. Kutas,et al.  Neural correlates of encoding in an incidental learning paradigm. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[19]  H G Vaughan,et al.  Association cortex potentials and reaction time in auditory discrimination. , 1972, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[20]  R. Shepard,et al.  Retention of information under conditions approaching a steady state. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[21]  David Friedman,et al.  P300 and slow wave: The effects of reaction time quartile , 1984, Biological Psychology.

[22]  C. C. Wood,et al.  Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. , 1985, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[23]  J. G. Snodgrass,et al.  Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: applications to dementia and amnesia. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[24]  K. Davis,et al.  Enhancement of memory processes in Alzheimer's disease with multiple-dose intravenous physostigmine. , 1982, The American journal of psychiatry.

[25]  L W Poon,et al.  Age and word frequency effects in continuous recognition memory. , 1980, Journal of gerontology.

[26]  E. Halgren,et al.  Human medial temporal lobe potentials evoked in memory and language tasks. , 1986, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[27]  D Friedman,et al.  Multiple late positive potentials in two visual discrimination tasks. , 1981, Psychophysiology.

[28]  T W Picton,et al.  Searching for the names of pictures: an event-related potential study. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[29]  C C Wood,et al.  Principal component analysis of event-related potentials: simulation studies demonstrate misallocation of variance across components. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[30]  Vaughan Hg,et al.  Analysis of electroencephalographic correlates of human sensori-motor processes. , 1968 .

[31]  S Sutton,et al.  ERP components in picture matching in children and adults. , 1988, Psychophysiology.

[32]  M. Kutas,et al.  Event-related brain potentials during initial encoding and recognition memory of congruous and incongruous words , 1986 .

[33]  Daniel S. Ruchkin,et al.  11 Positive Slow Wave and P300: Association and Disassociation , 1983 .

[34]  A. D. Smith,et al.  Effects of age and a divided attention task presented during encoding and retrieval on memory. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[35]  N. Squires,et al.  Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[36]  M. Rugg,et al.  Lexical contribution to nonword-repetition effects: Evidence from event-related potentials , 1987, Memory & cognition.

[37]  R. Shepard Recognition memory for words, sentences, and pictures , 1967 .

[38]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. , 1977, Science.

[39]  M. Rugg The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[40]  Shlomo Bentin,et al.  Event-related potentials, semantic processes, and expectancy factors in word recognition , 1987, Brain and Language.

[41]  R. Nickerson,et al.  SHORT-TERM MEMORY FOR COMPLEX MEANINGFUL VISUAL CONFIGURATIONS: A DEMONSTRATION OF CAPACITY. , 1965, Canadian journal of psychology.

[42]  D. Stuss,et al.  Event-related potentials during naming and mental rotation. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.