Can the theory of “whitening” explain the center-surround properties of retinal ganglion cell receptive fields?

To account for the spatial and temporal response properties of the retina, a number of studies have proposed that these properties serve to "whiten" the visual input. In particular, it has been argued that the sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells is matched to the spatial frequency spectrum of natural scenes, resulting in a flattened or "whitened" response spectrum across a range of frequencies. However, we argue that there are two distinct hypotheses regarding the flattening of the spectrum. The decorrelation hypothesis proposes that the magnitude of each ganglion cell tuning curve rises with spatial frequency, resulting in a flattened response spectrum for natural scene stimuli. With appropriate sampling, this scheme allows neighboring neurons to be uncorrelated with each other. The response equalization hypothesis proposes that the overall response magnitude of neurons increases with spatial frequency. The proposed goal of this model is to allow neurons with different receptive field sizes to produce the same average response to natural scenes. The response equalization hypothesis proposes an explanation for the relative gain of different ganglion cells and we show that this proposal fits well with published data. We suggest that both hypotheses are important in understanding the tuning and sensitivity of ganglion cells. However, using a simulation, both models are shown to be insufficient to explain the center-surround receptive field organization of ganglion cells. We discuss other factors, including representational sparseness, which could be related to the goals of ganglion cell spatial processing. We suggest three constraints needed to describe the basic linear properties of P-type ganglion cells: decorrelation, response equalization, and a minimal wiring or minimal size constraint.

[1]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The receptive field of the primate P retinal ganglion cell, I: Linear dynamics , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[2]  Joseph J. Atick,et al.  What Does the Retina Know about Natural Scenes? , 1992, Neural Computation.

[3]  R. Shapley,et al.  Nonlinear spatial summation and the contrast gain control of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1979, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  J. H. Hateren,et al.  Independent component filters of natural images compared with simple cells in primary visual cortex , 1998 .

[5]  D J Field,et al.  Relations between the statistics of natural images and the response properties of cortical cells. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[6]  Ethan A. Benardete,et al.  Chapter 2 The dynamics of primate retinal ganglion cells , 2001 .

[7]  L. Croner,et al.  Receptive fields of P and M ganglion cells across the primate retina , 1995, Vision Research.

[8]  G. Mitchison Neuronal branching patterns and the economy of cortical wiring , 1991, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  D. Field,et al.  What's constant in contrast constancy? The effects of scaling on the perceived contrast of bandpass patterns , 1995, Vision Research.

[10]  D. Baylor,et al.  Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  David J. Field,et al.  Emergence of simple-cell receptive field properties by learning a sparse code for natural images , 1996, Nature.

[12]  M. Meister,et al.  1SD56 Dynamic Predictive Coding by the Retina , 2005 .

[13]  J. Atick,et al.  Temporal decorrelation: a theory of lagged and nonlagged responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus , 1995 .

[14]  J D Victor,et al.  How the contrast gain control modifies the frequency responses of cat retinal ganglion cells. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[15]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The dynamics of primate M retinal ganglion cells , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.

[16]  G. R. Wyatt Comprehensive insect physiology, biochemistry and pharmacology , 1985 .

[17]  B. Finlay,et al.  Comparative Aspects of Visual System Development , 2006 .

[18]  K. Naka,et al.  S‐potentials from colour units in the retina of fish (Cyprinidae) , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  R. Wong,et al.  Retinal waves and visual system development. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.

[20]  J. Atick,et al.  STATISTICS OF NATURAL TIME-VARYING IMAGES , 1995 .

[21]  D. Jameson,et al.  Mach bands : quantitative studies on neural networks in the retina , 1966 .

[22]  D. Mastronarde Correlated firing of retinal ganglion cells , 1989, Trends in Neurosciences.

[23]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[24]  Richard H. Masland,et al.  Receptive Field Microstructure and Dendritic Geometry of Retinal Ganglion Cells , 2000, Neuron.

[25]  E. Kaplan,et al.  The receptive field of the primate P retinal ganglion cell, II: Nonlinear dynamics , 1997, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  Terrence J. Sejnowski,et al.  The “independent components” of natural scenes are edge filters , 1997, Vision Research.

[27]  Dawei W. Dong,et al.  Spatiotemporal Inseparability of Natural Images and Visual Sensitivities , 2001 .

[28]  J. Victor The dynamics of the cat retinal X cell centre. , 1987, The Journal of physiology.

[29]  M W Levine,et al.  Correlation of activity in neighbouring goldfish ganglion cells: relationship between latency and lag. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[30]  S. Laughlin,et al.  Predictive coding: a fresh view of inhibition in the retina , 1982, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[31]  D. Field,et al.  Visual sensitivity, blur and the sources of variability in the amplitude spectra of natural scenes , 1997, Vision Research.

[32]  D J Field,et al.  Local Contrast in Natural Images: Normalisation and Coding Efficiency , 2000, Perception.

[33]  S. DeVries Correlated firing in rabbit retinal ganglion cells. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  T E Spraker,et al.  Cross‐correlation analysis of the maintained discharge of rabbit retinal ganglion cells. , 1981, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  F. Werblin,et al.  Control of Retinal Sensitivity: I. Light and Dark Adaptation of Vertebrate Rods and Cones , 1974 .

[36]  Michael J. Berry,et al.  The structure and precision of retinal spike trains. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[37]  M. Meister Multineuronal codes in retinal signaling. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  E Kaplan,et al.  The dynamics of primate retinal ganglion cells. , 2001, Progress in brain research.

[39]  G. J. Burton,et al.  Color and spatial structure in natural scenes. , 1987, Applied optics.

[40]  R. Jampel Mach Bands, Quantitative Studies on Neural Networks in the Retina. , 1966 .

[41]  David J. Field,et al.  Wavelets, blur, and the sources of variability in the amplitude spectra of natural scenes , 1996, Electronic Imaging.

[42]  R. Reid,et al.  Temporal Coding of Visual Information in the Thalamus , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  G. A. Kerkut,et al.  Comprehensive insect physiology, biochemistry, and pharmacology , 1985 .

[44]  Roland J. Baddeley,et al.  Synaptic energy efficiency in retinal processing , 2003, Vision Research.

[45]  R. W. Rodieck Quantitative analysis of cat retinal ganglion cell response to visual stimuli. , 1965, Vision research.

[46]  David J. Field,et al.  What Is the Goal of Sensory Coding? , 1994, Neural Computation.

[47]  N M Grzywacz,et al.  The role of early retinal lateral inhibition: More than maximizing luminance information , 2000, Visual Neuroscience.

[48]  Ralph Linsker,et al.  How to Generate Ordered Maps by Maximizing the Mutual Information between Input and Output Signals , 1989, Neural Computation.

[49]  D. Baylor,et al.  Concerted Signaling by Retinal Ganglion Cells , 1995, Science.

[50]  D. Arnett Statistical dependence between neighboring retinal ganglion cells in goldfish , 1978, Experimental Brain Research.