Preverbal infants utilize cross-modal semantic congruency in artificial grammar acquisition

[1]  R. Kana,et al.  The impact of atypical sensory processing on social impairments in autism spectrum disorder , 2017, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Terry K Koo,et al.  A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. , 2016, Journal of chiropractic medicine.

[3]  Brock Ferguson,et al.  Communicative signals support abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[4]  Yuen Ki Ma,et al.  Bimodal emotion congruency is critical to preverbal infants' abstract rule learning. , 2016, Developmental science.

[5]  Markus Huff,et al.  Semantic congruency but not temporal synchrony enhances long-term memory performance for audio-visual scenes , 2016, Memory & cognition.

[6]  L. Bahrick,et al.  A systems view of mother-infant face-to-face communication. , 2016, Developmental psychology.

[7]  Micah M. Murray,et al.  Single-trial multisensory memories affect later auditory and visual object discrimination , 2015, Cognition.

[8]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Spatio‐temporal dynamics of adaptation in the human visual system: a high‐density electrical mapping study , 2015, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  K. Hiraki,et al.  Cortical EEG components that reflect inverse effectiveness during visuotactile integration processing , 2015, Brain Research.

[10]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Learning Stimulus-Location Associations in 8- and 11-Month-Old Infants: Multimodal versus Unimodal Information. , 2014, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[11]  D. Maurer,et al.  Perceptual narrowing during infancy: a comparison of language and faces. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[12]  D. Lewkowicz Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[13]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  Infants Are Not Sensitive to Synesthetic Cross-Modality Correspondences , 2014, Psychological science.

[14]  Arlette Streri,et al.  The foundations of social cognition: Studies on face/voice integration in newborn infants , 2013 .

[15]  A. Vouloumanos,et al.  Sound symbolism in infancy: evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-month-olds. , 2013, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[16]  Erik D. Thiessen Effects of Inter- and Intra-modal Redundancy on Infants' Rule Learning , 2012 .

[17]  Kevin A Hallgren,et al.  Computing Inter-Rater Reliability for Observational Data: An Overview and Tutorial. , 2012, Tutorials in quantitative methods for psychology.

[18]  Megan A. K. Peters,et al.  0 + 1 > 1 , 2012, Psychological science.

[19]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory interactions in early evoked brain activity follow the principle of inverse effectiveness , 2011, NeuroImage.

[20]  Marcia Grabowecky,et al.  Object-based auditory facilitation of visual search for pictures and words with frequent and rare targets. , 2011, Acta psychologica.

[21]  M. Grabowecky,et al.  Characteristic sounds make you look at target objects more quickly , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[22]  S. Carey,et al.  The long and the short of it: On the nature and origin of functional overlap between representations of space and time , 2010, Cognition.

[23]  C. Spence,et al.  When hearing the bark helps to identify the dog: Semantically-congruent sounds modulate the identification of masked pictures , 2010, Cognition.

[24]  Clara Casco,et al.  Audiovisual bounce-inducing effect: When sound congruence affects grouping in vision , 2010, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[25]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Preverbal Infants’ Sensitivity to Synaesthetic Cross-Modality Correspondences , 2010, Psychological science.

[26]  Marina Nespor,et al.  Perceptual and memory constraints on language acquisition , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Information from multiple modalities helps 5-month-olds learn abstract rules. , 2009, Developmental science.

[28]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  From domain-generality to domain-sensitivity: 4-Month-olds learn an abstract repetition rule in music that 7-month-olds do not , 2009, Cognition.

[29]  C. G. Prince,et al.  Two-month-old infants' sensitivity to changes in arbitrary syllable-object pairings: the role of temporal synchrony. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Michael C. Frank,et al.  Abstract Rule Learning for Visual Sequences in 8- and 11-Month-Olds. , 2009, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[31]  M. Grabowecky,et al.  Characteristic sounds facilitate visual search , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Benefits of Stimulus Congruency for Multisensory Facilitation of Visual Learning , 2008, PloS one.

[33]  Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz,et al.  Perceptual constraints and the learnability of simple grammars , 2007, Cognition.

[34]  J. Saffran,et al.  Dog is a dog is a dog: Infant rule learning is not specific to language , 2007, Cognition.

[35]  Edgar Erdfelder,et al.  G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences , 2007, Behavior research methods.

[36]  Morten H. Christiansen,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Statistical Learning Within and Between Modalities Pitting Abstract Against Stimulus-Specific Representations , 2022 .

[37]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Do you see what I am saying? Exploring visual enhancement of speech comprehension in noisy environments. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.

[38]  T. Striano,et al.  Crossmodal integration of emotional information from face and voice in the infant brain. , 2006, Developmental science.

[39]  Jacques Mehler,et al.  The role of salience in the extraction of algebraic rules. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[40]  M. Murray,et al.  The role of multisensory memories in unisensory object discrimination. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[41]  Paul J. Laurienti,et al.  Semantic congruence is a critical factor in multisensory behavioral performance , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[42]  D. Lewkowicz Perception of serial order in infants. , 2004, Developmental science.

[43]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Multisensory visual-auditory object recognition in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[44]  Christoph M. Michel,et al.  Rapid discrimination of visual and multisensory memories revealed by electrical neuroimaging , 2004, NeuroImage.

[45]  J. Werker,et al.  Two-month-old infants match phonetic information in lips and voice , 2003 .

[46]  Robert Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[47]  L. Gogate,et al.  Intersensory Redundancy and 7-Month-Old Infants' Memory for Arbitrary Syllable-Object Relations , 2001 .

[48]  L. Gogate,et al.  A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. , 2000, Child development.

[49]  R. Lickliter,et al.  Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. , 2000, Developmental psychology.

[50]  Alan Slater,et al.  Intermodal perception at birth: Intersensory redundancy guides newborn infants’ learning of arbitrary auditory−visual pairings , 1999 .

[51]  Elizabeth K. Johnson,et al.  Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults , 1999, Cognition.

[52]  Tiffany M Field,et al.  Full-term and preterm infants' perception of face-voice synchrony , 1994 .

[53]  L. Bahrick The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Arbitrary Intermodal Relations , 1994 .

[54]  L. Bahrick,et al.  Intermodal learning in infancy: learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relations in audible and visible events. , 1988, Child development.

[55]  B. Stein,et al.  Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[56]  L. Bahrick Infants' intermodal perception of two levels of temporal structure in natural events☆ , 1987 .

[57]  A. Walker-Andrews Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors: Relation of eye and voice? , 1986 .

[58]  E. Spelke,et al.  Detection of intermodal numerical correspondences by human infants. , 1983, Science.

[59]  A. Walker,et al.  Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors by human infants. , 1982, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[60]  G. Turkewitz,et al.  Intersensory function in newborns: effect of sound on visual preferences. , 1980, Child development.

[61]  David J. Lewkowicz,et al.  Cross-modal equivalence in early infancy: Auditory–visual intensity matching. , 1980 .

[62]  E. Spelke Perceiving Bimodally Specified Events in Infancy , 1979 .

[63]  Kaisa Tiippana,et al.  Audiovisual semantic congruency during encoding enhances memory performance. , 2015, Experimental psychology.

[64]  Madhavilatha Maganti,et al.  Cross-cultural evidence for multimodal motherese: Asian Indian mothers' adaptive use of synchronous words and gestures. , 2015, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[65]  R Core Team,et al.  R: A language and environment for statistical computing. , 2014 .

[66]  Ladan Shams,et al.  Multisensory encoding improves auditory recognition. , 2013, Multisensory research.

[67]  Megan A. K. Peters,et al.  How Adding Noninformative Sound Improves Performance on a Visual Task , 2011 .

[68]  A. Rodríguez-Fornells,et al.  Words as anchors: known words facilitate statistical learning. , 2010, Experimental psychology.

[69]  L. Bahrick,et al.  The development of infant discrimination of affect in multimodal and unimodal stimulation: The role of intersensory redundancy. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[70]  J. Werker,et al.  Infants' ability to match dynamic phonetic and gender information in the face and voice. , 2002, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[71]  Peter M. Vishton,et al.  Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. , 1999, Science.

[72]  B. Morrongiello,et al.  Developmental changes in associations between auditory-visual events , 1998 .

[73]  B. Morrongiello,et al.  Crossmodal learning in newborn infants: Inferences about properties of auditory-visual events , 1998 .

[74]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[75]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.