Creating flexible motor memories in human walking

The human nervous system has the ability to save newly learned movements (i.e. re-learn faster after initial learning) and generalize learning to new conditions. In the context of walking, we rely on savings and generalization of newly learned walking patterns to navigate changing environments and make progressive improvements with gait rehabilitation. Here, we used a split-belt treadmill to study how different perturbation parameters can influence savings and generalization of learning during walking. In Experiment 1, we investigated the effect of split perturbation size on savings of a newly learned walking pattern. We found that larger perturbations led to better savings than smaller perturbations. In Experiment 2, we studied how different features of the initial split perturbation influenced the generalization of learning. Interestingly, we found that practicing the same thing twice did not lead to fastest learning. Instead, initial exposure to larger perturbation ratios led to faster subsequent learning of smaller perturbation ratios as compared to repeated exposures to small perturbations. Collectively, our findings reveal that initial learning conditions can be leveraged to increase savings and shape flexible motor memories during walking.

[1]  M. Mauk,et al.  A Mechanism for Savings in the Cerebellum , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  J R Flanagan,et al.  Composition and Decomposition of Internal Models in Motor Learning under Altered Kinematic and Dynamic Environments , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[3]  T. Drew,et al.  Taking the next step: cortical contributions to the control of locomotion , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[4]  Philippe Lefèvre,et al.  Formation of model-free motor memories during motor adaptation depends on perturbation schedule. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Motor Adaptation Training for Faster Relearning , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  A memory of errors in sensorimotor learning , 2014, Science.

[7]  Yi-Chung Clive Pai,et al.  Intensity and generalization of treadmill slip training: High or low, progressive increase or decrease? , 2016, Journal of biomechanics.

[8]  B. Fajen,et al.  Humans exploit the biomechanics of bipedal gait during visually guided walking over complex terrain , 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[9]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  Interacting Adaptive Processes with Different Timescales Underlie Short-Term Motor Learning , 2006, PLoS biology.

[10]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Hannah J. Block,et al.  Interlimb coordination during locomotion: what can be adapted and stored? , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Throwing while looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[13]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Split-belt treadmill adaptation shows different functional networks for fast and slow human walking. , 2010, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  Brett R Fajen,et al.  Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[15]  O. Bock,et al.  Mechanisms for sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[16]  Peter J Beek,et al.  Speeding up or slowing down?: Gait adaptations to preserve gait stability in response to balance perturbations. , 2012, Gait & posture.

[17]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[18]  Matthew J. Crossley,et al.  Savings upon Re-Aiming in Visuomotor Adaptation , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[19]  Denise Y. P. Henriques,et al.  Visuomotor adaptation and generalization with repeated and varied training , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Conditions for interference versus facilitation during sequential sensorimotor adaptation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Hannah E Smithson,et al.  Different aberrations raise contrast thresholds for single-letter identification in line with their effect on cross-correlation-based confusability. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[22]  R. Seidler Multiple Motor Learning Experiences Enhance Motor Adaptability , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Amy J Bastian,et al.  Two ways to save a newly learned motor pattern. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Daniel A. Braun,et al.  Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning , 2009, Current Biology.

[25]  P. Bédard,et al.  Attention modulates generalization of visuomotor adaptation. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[26]  Krista M Bond,et al.  Flexible explicit but rigid implicit learning in a visuomotor adaptation task. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[27]  A. Haith,et al.  Unlearning versus savings in visuomotor adaptation: comparing effects of washout, passage of time, and removal of errors on motor memory , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..