Efficient Visual Search from Synchronized Auditory Signals Requires Transient Audiovisual Events
暂无分享,去创建一个
Jan Theeuwes | Christian N. L. Olivers | David Alais | John Cass | Erik Van der Burg | D. Alais | J. Cass | E. Van der Burg | J. Theeuwes | C. Olivers | Erik Van der Burg
[1] G. J. Thomas. Experimental study of the influence of vision on sound localization. , 1941 .
[2] B. Stein,et al. Spatial factors determine the activity of multisensory neurons in cat superior colliculus , 1986, Brain Research.
[3] J. Rieger,et al. Audiovisual Temporal Correspondence Modulates Human Multisensory Superior Temporal Sulcus Plus Primary Sensory Cortices , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[4] Mark D'Esposito,et al. Searching for “the Top” in Top-Down Control , 2005, Neuron.
[5] S. Shimojo,et al. Illusions: What you see is what you hear , 2000, Nature.
[6] M. Wallace,et al. Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.
[7] G. Calvert,et al. Multisensory integration: methodological approaches and emerging principles in the human brain , 2004, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[8] H. McGurk,et al. Hearing lips and seeing voices , 1976, Nature.
[9] D. Burr,et al. Combining visual and auditory information. , 2006, Progress in brain research.
[10] B. Stein,et al. Enhancement of Perceived Visual Intensity by Auditory Stimuli: A Psychophysical Analysis , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[11] Linda Lundström,et al. The pupils and optical systems of gecko eyes. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[12] S. Nishida,et al. Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.
[13] E. Van der Burg,et al. Audiovisual events capture attention: evidence from temporal order judgments. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[14] T. Stanford,et al. Multisensory integration: current issues from the perspective of the single neuron , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[15] R. Campbell,et al. Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging of crossmodal binding in the human heteromodal cortex , 2000, Current Biology.
[16] J. Vroomen,et al. Sound enhances visual perception: cross-modal effects of auditory organization on vision. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[17] S. Zeki. Functional specialisation in the visual cortex of the rhesus monkey , 1978, Nature.
[18] Mikko Sams,et al. Perceiving identical sounds as speech or non-speech modulates activity in the left posterior superior temporal sulcus , 2006, NeuroImage.
[19] Daniel Senkowski,et al. Good times for multisensory integration: Effects of the precision of temporal synchrony as revealed by gamma-band oscillations , 2007, Neuropsychologia.
[20] Akihiro Yagi,et al. Reduction of stimulus visibility compresses apparent time intervals , 2008, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] Rainer Goebel,et al. Top–down task effects overrule automatic multisensory responses to letter–sound pairs in auditory association cortex , 2006, NeuroImage.
[22] John J. Foxe,et al. The timing and laminar profile of converging inputs to multisensory areas of the macaque neocortex. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[23] M. Giard,et al. Auditory-Visual Integration during Multimodal Object Recognition in Humans: A Behavioral and Electrophysiological Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[24] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. Visual search for a target changing in synchrony with an auditory signal , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[25] A. Kingstone,et al. Auditory capture of vision: examining temporal ventriloquism. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[26] P. Bertelson,et al. The ventriloquist effect does not depend on the direction of automatic visual attention , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] Waka Fujisaki,et al. Temporal frequency characteristics of synchrony–asynchrony discrimination of audio-visual signals , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] J. Theeuwes,et al. Poke and pop: Tactile–visual synchrony increases visual saliency , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.
[29] T SHIPLEY,et al. Auditory Flutter-Driving of Visual Flicker , 1964, Science.
[30] Rainer Goebel,et al. The effect of temporal asynchrony on the multisensory integration of letters and speech sounds. , 2006, Cerebral cortex.
[31] Sidney S. Simon,et al. Merging of the Senses , 2008, Front. Neurosci..
[32] S. Yantis,et al. Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: evidence from visual search. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[33] C. Olivers,et al. Bleeping you out of the blink: Sound saves vision from oblivion , 2008, Brain Research.
[34] D. Burr,et al. The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration , 2004, Current Biology.
[35] J. Driver,et al. Audiovisual links in exogenous covert spatial orienting , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[36] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Pip and pop: nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] Shin'ya Nishida,et al. The sliding window of audio-visual simultaneity. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[38] Piotr Jaśkowski,et al. Temporal-order judgments and reaction time for stimuli of different modalities , 1990, Psychological research.
[39] H. Bülthoff,et al. Merging the senses into a robust percept , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[40] S. Nishida,et al. Audio–tactile superiority over visuo–tactile and audio–visual combinations in the temporal resolution of synchrony perception , 2009, Experimental Brain Research.
[41] J. Lewald,et al. Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.