Dissociable Neural Information Dynamics of Perceptual Integration and Differentiation during Bistable Perception

Abstract At any given moment, we experience a perceptual scene as a single whole and yet we may distinguish a variety of objects within it. This phenomenon instantiates two properties of conscious perception: integration and differentiation. Integration is the property of experiencing a collection of objects as a unitary percept and differentiation is the property of experiencing these objects as distinct from each other. Here, we evaluated the neural information dynamics underlying integration and differentiation of perceptual contents during bistable perception. Participants listened to a sequence of tones (auditory bistable stimuli) experienced either as a single stream (perceptual integration) or as two parallel streams (perceptual differentiation) of sounds. We computed neurophysiological indices of information integration and information differentiation with electroencephalographic and intracranial recordings. When perceptual alternations were endogenously driven, the integrated percept was associated with an increase in neural information integration and a decrease in neural differentiation across frontoparietal regions, whereas the opposite pattern was observed for the differentiated percept. However, when perception was exogenously driven by a change in the sound stream (no bistability), neural oscillatory power distinguished between percepts but information measures did not. We demonstrate that perceptual integration and differentiation can be mapped to theoretically motivated neural information signatures, suggesting a direct relationship between phenomenology and neurophysiology.

[1]  C. Stam,et al.  Phase lag index: Assessment of functional connectivity from multi channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[2]  W. Singer,et al.  Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience Integrative Neuroscience Neural Synchrony in Cortical Networks: History, Concept and Current Status , 2022 .

[3]  Guy B. Williams,et al.  Spectral Signatures of Reorganised Brain Networks in Disorders of Consciousness , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[4]  Melissa K. Gregg,et al.  Attention, Awareness, and the Perception of Auditory Scenes , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[5]  Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al.  Intracranial Recordings of Occipital Cortex Responses to Illusory Visual Events , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  M. Sigman,et al.  Auditory Feedback Differentially Modulates Behavioral and Neural Markers of Objective and Subjective Performance When Tapping to Your Heartbeat , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[7]  A. Engel,et al.  Intrinsic Coupling Modes: Multiscale Interactions in Ongoing Brain Activity , 2013, Neuron.

[8]  W. Einhäuser,et al.  Binocular Rivalry: Frontal Activity Relates to Introspection and Action But Not to Perception , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  J. Kelso,et al.  The Metastable Brain , 2014, Neuron.

[10]  Jennifer Wiley,et al.  What Are the Odds? A Practical Guide to Computing and Reporting Bayes Factors , 2014, J. Probl. Solving.

[11]  Rhodri Cusack,et al.  The Intraparietal Sulcus and Perceptual Organization , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  Geraint Rees,et al.  Magnetoencephalographic Correlates of Perceptual State During Auditory Bistability , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[13]  M. Fox,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging , 2007, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming , 2013, Current Biology.

[15]  David Salomon,et al.  Data Compression: The Complete Reference , 2006 .

[16]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Converging Intracranial Markers of Conscious Access , 2009, PLoS biology.

[17]  Lawrence M. Ward,et al.  Increased gamma-band synchrony precedes switching of conscious perceptual objects in binocular rivalry , 2005, Neuroreport.

[18]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  EEG functional connectivity metrics wPLI and wSMI account for distinct types of brain functional interactions , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[19]  Ole Jensen,et al.  Spontaneous local alpha oscillations predict motion‐induced blindness , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[20]  Biyu J. He,et al.  Spontaneous Neural Dynamics and Multi-scale Network Organization , 2016, Front. Syst. Neurosci..

[21]  C. Rorden,et al.  Stereotaxic display of brain lesions. , 2000, Behavioural neurology.

[22]  C. Koch,et al.  Integrated information theory: from consciousness to its physical substrate , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  Juan R. Vidal,et al.  Selective Neural Synchrony Suppression as a Forward Gatekeeper to Piecemeal Conscious Perception. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  Jean-Luc Schwartz,et al.  Parieto-frontal gamma band activity during the perceptual emergence of speech forms , 2008, NeuroImage.

[25]  G. J. Chaitin The berry paradox , 1995 .

[26]  C. Koch,et al.  Neural correlates of consciousness: progress and problems , 2016, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[27]  Andrea Bergmann,et al.  Statistical Parametric Mapping The Analysis Of Functional Brain Images , 2016 .

[28]  Walter J Freeman,et al.  Mechanism and significance of global coherence in scalp EEG , 2015, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[29]  M. Sigman,et al.  Large scale screening of neural signatures of consciousness in patients in a vegetative or minimally conscious state. , 2014, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  I. Winkler,et al.  Event-related potential correlates of sound organization: Early sensory and late cognitive effects , 2013, Biological Psychology.

[31]  Guido Nolte,et al.  Spectral fingerprints of large‐scale cortical dynamics during ambiguous motion perception , 2016, Human brain mapping.

[32]  F. Varela,et al.  Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[33]  J. Hupé,et al.  Temporal Dynamics of Auditory and Visual Bistability Reveal Common Principles of Perceptual Organization , 2006, Current Biology.

[34]  Larissa Albantakis,et al.  From the Phenomenology to the Mechanisms of Consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0 , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[35]  Yuichi Murai,et al.  Visualization of transient interfacial waves induced by spin-up of two immiscible fluid layers , 2010, J. Vis..

[36]  G. Rees,et al.  The Neural Bases of Multistable Perception , 2022 .

[37]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[38]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[39]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Toward a computational theory of conscious processing , 2014, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[40]  Valdas Noreika,et al.  Intrusions of a drowsy mind: neural markers of phenomenological unpredictability , 2015, Frontiers in Psychology.

[41]  I. Winkler,et al.  Auditory perceptual objects as generative models: Setting the stage for communication by sound , 2015, Brain and Language.

[42]  Gregory J. Chaitin The Berry paradox , 1995, Complex..

[43]  Yong He,et al.  BrainNet Viewer: A Network Visualization Tool for Human Brain Connectomics , 2013, PloS one.

[44]  J. Mendola,et al.  Individual peak gamma frequency predicts switch rate in perceptual rivalry , 2015, Human brain mapping.

[45]  W. Singer,et al.  Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top–down processing , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[46]  M. Boly,et al.  Complexity of Multi-Dimensional Spontaneous EEG Decreases during Propofol Induced General Anaesthesia , 2015, PloS one.

[47]  M. Scherg,et al.  Neuromagnetic Correlates of Streaming in Human Auditory Cortex , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[48]  Meng Wang,et al.  Optimal referencing for stereo-electroencephalographic (SEEG) recordings , 2018, NeuroImage.

[49]  J. Martinerie,et al.  Comparison of Hilbert transform and wavelet methods for the analysis of neuronal synchrony , 2001, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[50]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data , 2010, Comput. Intell. Neurosci..

[51]  Gregory J. Chaitin,et al.  Information-theoretic computation complexity , 1974, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[52]  Christopher W. Bishop,et al.  Pattern of BOLD signal in auditory cortex relates acoustic response to perceptual streaming , 2011, BMC Neuroscience.

[53]  R. Carlyon,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance Automaticity and Primacy of Auditory Streaming : Concurrent Subjective and Objective Measures , 2015 .

[54]  A. Seth,et al.  Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine, LSD and psilocybin , 2017, Scientific Reports.

[55]  Andrei N. Kolmogorov,et al.  Logical basis for information theory and probability theory , 1968, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.

[56]  G. Tononi,et al.  A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior , 2013, Science Translational Medicine.

[57]  Matthew H. Davis,et al.  Neural Decoding of Bistable Sounds Reveals an Effect of Intention on Perceptual Organization , 2017, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[58]  Anastasia V Flevaris,et al.  Neural substrates of perceptual integration during bistable object perception. , 2013, Journal of vision.

[59]  Walter J. Freeman,et al.  Neurodynamics: An Exploration in Mesoscopic Brain Dynamics , 2000, Perspectives in Neural Computing.

[60]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Information Sharing in the Brain Indexes Consciousness in Noncommunicative Patients , 2013, Current Biology.

[61]  Lauri Parkkonen,et al.  Early visual brain areas reflect the percept of an ambiguous scene , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[62]  W. Singer,et al.  Synchronization of Neural Activity across Cortical Areas Correlates with Conscious Perception , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[63]  Robert Oostenveld,et al.  An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias , 2011, NeuroImage.

[64]  J. Martinerie,et al.  The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[65]  Emiliano Ricciardi,et al.  EEG functional connectivity metrics wPLI and wSMI account for distinct types of brain functional interactions , 2018, Scientific Reports.

[66]  Daniel Arteaga,et al.  Brain mechanisms for simple perception and bistable perception , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[67]  Hinze Hogendoorn,et al.  Perceptual integration without conscious access , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[68]  Michael E J Masson,et al.  A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[69]  Jessica M. Foxton,et al.  Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[70]  R. Jindra Mass action in the nervous system W. J. Freeman, Academic Press, New York (1975), 489 pp., (hard covers). $34.50 , 1976, Neuroscience.

[71]  G. Tononi,et al.  Stimulus Set Meaningfulness and Neurophysiological Differentiation: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 2014, bioRxiv.

[72]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Large-scale cortical correlation structure of spontaneous oscillatory activity , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[73]  Johannes Jacobus Fahrenfort,et al.  Neuronal integration in visual cortex elevates face category tuning to conscious face perception , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[74]  Abraham Lempel,et al.  On the Complexity of Finite Sequences , 1976, IEEE Trans. Inf. Theory.