Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica).
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Guillermo Paz-y-Miño C,et al. Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance , 2004, Nature.
[2] D. Crews,et al. Spatial and reversal learning in congeneric lizards with different foraging strategies , 1999, Animal Behaviour.
[3] D. Leger. Comparative perspectives in modern psychology. , 1987, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.
[4] L. Day,et al. Use of position and feature cues in discrimination learning by the whiptail lizard (Cnemidophorus inornatus). , 2003, Journal of comparative psychology.
[5] David W. Bessemer,et al. Retention of Discriminations and an Analysis of Learning Set , 1971 .
[6] K. Strier. Behavioral Flexibility in Primates. Causes and Consequences , 2006, International journal of primatology.
[7] M. Bitterman. THE EVOLUTION OF INTELLIGENCE. , 1965, Scientific American.
[8] J. Warren. Reversal learning and the formation of learning sets by cats and rhesus monkeys. , 1966, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology.
[9] Performance of Four Seed-Caching Corvid Species in the Radial-Arm Maze Analog , 1994 .
[10] B. Doty,et al. Reversal Learning of Object and Positional Discriminations by Mink, Ferrets and Skunks , 1969 .
[11] Phyllis C. Lee. The Genesis of Animal Play. Testing the Limits, Gordon M. Burghardt. J. Wiley, Chichester (2005), Pp. xvi+501. Price £32.95 , 2005 .
[12] N. Mackintosh,et al. Factors underlying improvement in serial reversal learning. , 1968, Canadian journal of psychology.
[13] A. Bond,et al. Kea, Bird of Paradox: The Evolution and Behavior of a New Zealand Parrot , 1999 .
[14] W. J. Carmen,et al. Noncooperative breeding in the California scrub-jay , 2004 .
[15] Warren Jm. Reversal learning and the formation of learning sets by cats and rhesus monkeys. , 1966 .
[16] Tf Voogd,et al. Animal Cognition in Nature , 1998 .
[17] M. Bitterman. PHYLETIC DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING. , 1965, The American psychologist.
[18] B. Gibson,et al. The fine-grained spatial abilities of three seed-caching corvids , 2005, Learning & behavior.
[19] D. Evans,et al. Book Reviews: Insect Defenses. Adaptive Mechanisms and Strategies of Prey and Predators. , 1990 .
[20] Daniel Sol,et al. Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds , 2002, Animal Behaviour.
[21] L. Lefebvre,et al. Positive and negative correlates of feeding innovations in birds: evidence for limited modularity , 2003 .
[22] E Stüssi. [Biomechanics in sports]. , 1989, Schweizerische Rundschau fur Medizin Praxis = Revue suisse de medecine Praxis.
[23] A. Bond,et al. Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids , 2003, Animal Behaviour.
[24] D. Upton. The Management of Manufacturing Flexibility , 1994 .
[25] Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al. Both social and ecological factors predict ungulate brain size , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[26] N. Mackintosh. The psychology of animal learning , 1974 .
[27] PETER A BEDNEKOFF,et al. Clark's nutcracker spatial memory: many errors might not be due to forgetting , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[28] B. Monroe,et al. A World Checklist of Birds , 1993 .
[29] A. Townsend Peterson,et al. Western Scrub-Jay (Aphelocoma californica) , 2002 .
[30] N. Emery,et al. The cognitive neuroscience of social behaviour , 2005 .
[31] J. Warren. THE COMPARATIVE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING. , 1965, Annual review of psychology.
[32] P. Godfrey‐Smith. Environmental complexity and the evolution of cognition. , 2002 .
[33] K. Breland,et al. The misbehavior of organisms. , 1961 .
[34] Morten L Kringelbach,et al. Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction , 2003, NeuroImage.
[35] Serial reversal training and nonreversal shift learning. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[36] E. Bernays,et al. Insect Defenses: Adaptive Mechanisms and Strategies of Prey and Predators , 1991 .
[37] S. Baird,et al. The birds of North America , 1974 .
[38] A. Kamil,et al. A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence. , 1987, Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation.
[39] A C Kamil,et al. Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in operant tests of nonspatial and spatial memory. , 1995, Journal of comparative psychology.
[40] Charles H. Southwick,et al. Behavior of Nonhuman Primates , 1966, The Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine.
[41] W. Robinson. Predator-prey interactions, informational complexity, and the origins of intelligence , 1985 .
[42] N. Mackintosh,et al. Transfer of Serial Reversal Learning in the Pigeon , 1986 .
[43] A. Kamil,et al. Positive transfer from successive reversal training to learning set in Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata) , 1977 .
[44] R. Quesada,et al. Espinosa de los monteros , 2000 .
[45] Jody L. Lewis,et al. Interference effects in the memory for serially presented locations in Clark's nutcrackers, Nucifraga columbiana. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[46] J. E. Mazur,et al. Learning and Behavior , 1966 .
[47] HighWire Press. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London , 1781, The London Medical Journal.
[48] A. Easton. Behavioural flexibility, social learning, and the frontal cortex. , 2005 .
[49] J. Cracraft,et al. Intergeneric relationships of the new world Jays inferred from cytochrome b gene sequences , 1997 .
[50] P. Bednekoff,et al. Long-term spatial memory in four seed-caching corvid species , 1997, Animal Behaviour.
[51] Marcel Eens,et al. The evolution of hippocampus volume and brain size in relation to food hoarding in birds , 2004 .
[52] O. Güntürkün,et al. Selective deficits in reversal learning after neostriatum caudolaterale lesions in pigeons: Possible behavioral equivalencies to the mammalian prefrontal system , 1998, Behavioural Brain Research.
[53] K. Armitage,et al. Life history consequences of social complexity a comparative study of ground-dwelling sciurids , 1998 .
[54] E. Wasserman,et al. Comparative cognition : experimental explorations of animal intelligence , 2009 .
[55] J. Felsenstein. Phylogenies and the Comparative Method , 1985, The American Naturalist.
[56] The Neural Basis of Cognitive Flexibility in Birds , 2009 .
[57] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[58] G. Keppel,et al. Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook , 1976 .
[59] E. Martins. The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology, Paul H. Harvey, Mark D. Pagel. Oxford University Press, Oxford (1991), vii, + 239 Price $24.95 paperback , 1992 .
[60] Cecilia Heyes,et al. Evolution of Cognition , 2001 .
[61] L. Lefebvre,et al. Brains, Innovations and Evolution in Birds and Primates , 2004, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[62] K. Laland,et al. Animal innovation: An introduction. , 2003 .
[63] L. Lefebvre,et al. Relative Size of the Hyperstriatum ventrale Is the Best Predictor of Feeding Innovation Rate in Birds , 2000, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[64] N. Emery. Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence , 2006, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[65] A. Kamil,et al. The Ecology and Evolution of Spatial Memory in Corvids of the Southwestern USA: The Perplexing Pinyon Jay , 1998 .
[66] O. Güntürkün,et al. Impaired learning of a color reversal task after NMDA receptor blockade in the pigeon (Columba livia) associative forebrain (neostriatum caudolaterale). , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.