Countermanding saccades in humans

We used a countermanding paradigm to investigate the relationship between conflicting cues for controlling human saccades. Subjects made a saccade to a target appearing suddenly in the periphery; but on some trials, after a delay, a stop-signal was presented that instructed subjects to inhibit the saccade. As we increased this delay, subjects increasingly failed to inhibit the movement. From measurements of this relationship, and of saccadic latency in control trials, we estimated the average time needed to inhibit the saccade (the stop-signal reaction time or SSRT). SSRTs were similar across subjects, between 125 and 145 ms, and did not vary with target luminance. We then investigated a race model in which the target initiates a response preparation signal rising linearly with a rate varying randomly from trial to trial, and racing against a similarly rising signal initiated by the cue to inhibit the saccade. The first process to cross a trigger threshold determines whether the saccade is initiated or not. In Monte Carlo simulations, this model correctly predicted the probability of successful saccade inhibition as a function of the stop-signal delay, and also the statistical distributions of saccadic latency during trials in which a stop-signal was presented but the subject failed to inhibit the saccade. These findings provide a comparison to results previously described in the monkey, and show that a simple race model with a linear rise to threshold may underlie behavioural performance in tasks of this kind.

[1]  G D Logan,et al.  On the autonomy of mental processes: a case study of arithmetic. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[2]  J. Schall,et al.  Role of frontal eye fields in countermanding saccades: visual, movement, and fixation activity. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[4]  G. Logan,et al.  In search of the point of no return: the control of response processes. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[5]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit complex movements: A stop-signal study of typewriting. , 1982 .

[6]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[7]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Saccade-related activity in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of burst and buildup cells. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[8]  G. Westheimer Eye movement responses to a horizontally moving visual stimulus. , 1954, A.M.A. archives of ophthalmology.

[9]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[10]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. II. Reversible activation and deactivation. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[12]  J. Schall Visuomotor Areas of the Frontal Lobe , 1997 .

[13]  D. Meyer,et al.  The point of no return in choice reaction time: controlled and ballistic stages of response preparation. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[14]  D. F. Fisher,et al.  Eye movements : cognition and visual perception , 1982 .

[15]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[17]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Fixation cells in monkey superior colliculus. I. Characteristics of cell discharge. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  G D Logan,et al.  Strategies and mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitory motor control. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  W. Becker,et al.  An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli , 1979, Vision Research.

[20]  L. Festinger,et al.  Some observations concerning saccadic eye movements. , 1973, Vision research.

[21]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Activity of superior colliculus in behaving monkey. I. Visual receptive fields of single neurons. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[22]  M. Vince The intermittency of control movements and the psychological refractory period. , 1948, The British journal of psychology. General section.

[23]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[24]  S Kornblum,et al.  Does motor programming necessitate response execution? , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Role of the rostral superior colliculus in active visual fixation and execution of express saccades. , 1992, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Use of a delayed signal to stop a visual reaction-time response. , 1966 .

[27]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Effect of mean reaction time on saccadic responses to two-step stimuli with horizontal and vertical components , 1975, Vision Research.

[28]  L L Wheeless,et al.  Eye-movement responses to step and pulse-step stimuli. , 1966, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[29]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[30]  J. Schall,et al.  Saccade latency in context: Regulation of gaze behavior by supplementary eye field , 1993, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.