Interruptions create prospective memory tasks

When the theory of prospective memory is brought to bear on the ubiquitous experience of failing to resume interrupted tasks, the cognitive reasons for these failures may be understood and addressed. We examine three features of interruptions that may account for these failures: (1) Interruptions often abruptly divert attention, which may prevent adequate encoding of an intention to resume and forming an implementation plan, (2) New task demands after an interruption's end reduce opportunity to interpret resumption cues, (3) The transition after an interruption to new ongoing task demands is not distinctive because it is defined conceptually, rather than by a single perceptual cue. Hypotheses based on these three features receive support from two experiments that respectively manipulate encoding and retrieval conditions. The data support our contention that interrupted tasks are a special case of prospective memory, and allow us to suggest practical ways of reducing vulnerability to resumption failure. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

[1]  Martin L. Bink,et al.  Breaks and task switches in prospective memory , 2006 .

[2]  Brian P. Bailey,et al.  The Effects of Interruptions on Task Performance, Annoyance, and Anxiety in the User Interface , 2001, INTERACT.

[3]  R. Marsh,et al.  Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[4]  L. Kvavilashvili,et al.  Is time-based prospective remembering mediated by self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental cues, ongoing activity, age, and motivation. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[5]  W. Cordell,et al.  Emergency department workplace interruptions: are emergency physicians "interrupt-driven" and "multitasking"? , 2000, Academic emergency medicine : official journal of the Society for Academic Emergency Medicine.

[6]  S. L. Richardson,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: examining the influences of self-initiated retrieval processes. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[7]  Gilles O. Einstein,et al.  Prospective memory: When reminders fail , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[8]  K. Dismukes,et al.  Prospective Memory, Concurrent Task Management, and Pilot Error , 2007 .

[9]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Habitual Prospective Memory and Aging: Remembering Intentions and Forgetting Actions , 1998 .

[10]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework , 2000 .

[11]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Memory for goals: an activation-based model , 2002, Cogn. Sci..

[12]  P Shaw,et al.  Aging and prospective memory: the influence of increased task demands at encoding and retrieval. , 1997, Psychology and aging.

[13]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[14]  M. McDaniel,et al.  (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1002 Delaying Execution of Intentions: Overcoming the Costs of Interruptions , 2022 .

[15]  M. Brandimonte,et al.  The Effect of Cue-Familiarity, Cue-Distinctiveness, and Retention Interval on Prospective Remembering , 1994, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[16]  Mary Czerwinski,et al.  Instant Messaging and Interruption: Influence of Task Type on Performance , 2000 .

[17]  J. Rönnberg,et al.  Do elderly couples have a better prospective memory than other elderly people when they collaborate , 2000 .

[18]  Robert L Sumwalt,et al.  Cockpit Interruptions and Distractions: Effective Management Requires a Careful Balancing Act , 1999 .

[19]  P. Graf,et al.  Prospective Memory: A New Focus for Research , 2001, Consciousness and Cognition.

[20]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective memory and aging: forgetting intentions over short delays. , 2000, Psychology and aging.

[21]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Normal aging and prospective memory. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[22]  Matihias Kijegel Plan formation , retention , and execution in prospective memory : A new approach and age-related effects , 2010 .

[23]  R. Marsh,et al.  The properties of retention intervals and their affect on retaining prospective memories. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  D. Burrows,et al.  Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition (SARMAC) , 1997 .

[25]  Christopher A. Monk,et al.  Recovering From Interruptions: Implications for Driver Distraction Research , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[26]  Mark A McDaniel,et al.  Aging and maintaining intentions over delays: do it or lose it. , 2003, Psychology and aging.

[27]  Martin L. Bink,et al.  Activation of completed, uncompleted, and partially completed intentions , 1998 .

[28]  M. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective Memory: An Overview and Synthesis of an Emerging Field , 2007 .

[29]  P. Gollwitzer,et al.  Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit , 1997 .

[30]  F. Craik,et al.  Virtual week and actual week: Age‐related differences in prospective memory , 2000 .

[31]  Judi A. Ellis,et al.  Conceptual and perceptual processes in prospective remembering: Differential influence of attentional resources , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[32]  M. B. Edwards,et al.  Task interruption and its effects on memory. , 1998, Memory.

[33]  Brian P. Bailey,et al.  If not now, when?: the effects of interruption at different moments within task execution , 2004, CHI.

[34]  R. Marsh,et al.  An Analysis of Prospective Memory , 2006 .

[35]  O. U. Vortac,et al.  Functions of external cues in prospective memory. , 1995, Memory.

[36]  F. Craik,et al.  Age-related decline in prospective memory: the roles of cue accessibility and cue sensitivity. , 1999, Psychology and aging.

[37]  T. Mäntylä,et al.  Interrupting intentions: Zeigarnik-like effects in prospective memory , 1997, Psychological research.

[38]  Key Dismukes,et al.  A Task Interrupted Becomes A Prospective Task , 2003 .

[39]  J. Gregory Trafton,et al.  Preparing to resume an interrupted task: effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal , 2003, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[40]  Joseph S. Valacich,et al.  The Influence of Task Interruption on Individual Decision Making: An Information Overload Perspective , 1999 .

[41]  Sergio Della Sala,et al.  Working memory in event‐ and time‐based prospective memory tasks: Effects of secondary demand and age , 2004 .

[42]  T. Mäntylä,et al.  Priming effects in prospective memory. , 1993, Memory.

[43]  Mark A. McDaniel,et al.  Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptually driven processes? , 1998, Memory & cognition.

[44]  E A Maylor Aging and forgetting in prospective and retrospective memory tasks. , 1993, Psychology and aging.

[45]  Visual memory support: an effective mnemonic device for older adults. , 1996, The Gerontologist.

[46]  R Key Dismukes,et al.  The Limits of Expertise: Rethinking Pilot Error and the Causes of Airline Accidents , 2007 .

[47]  Mark A McDaniel,et al.  Cue-focused and reflexive-associative processes in prospective memory retrieval. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.