The contribution of proprioceptive feedback to sensorimotor adaptation

It is known that proprioceptive signals from muscles, joints, and skin are involved in the execution of aimed arm movements, but their role in the acquisition of new motor behaviour is largely unknown. Previous research using deafferented patients yielded inconsistent findings: sensorimotor adaptation was found to be less, equal, or even better than in controls. The present study uses a different approach: we degraded proprioceptive signals in healthy subjects by wrist vibration, while leaving them intact in a control group. In order to induce sensorimotor adaptation, both groups executed pointing movements under a visual (Exp. A) or a mechanical perturbation (Exp. B). We found that adaptation to the visual distortion was little affected by vibration, while adaptation to the mechanical distortion was substantially impaired. We conclude that proprioceptive signals which overtly disagree with visual ones (Exp. A) are not used for adaptation, while those which largely agree with visual ones (Exp. B) are used to enhance adaptive recalibration. These results indicate that intact proprioception is needed for mechanical but not for visual adaptation, which implies that the underlying mechanisms are at least partly distinct.

[1]  O. Bock,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation to rotated visual input: different mechanisms for small versus large rotations , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[2]  G. Stratton Vision without inversion of the retinal image. , 1897 .

[3]  Jean-Louis Vercher,et al.  Wrist vibration affects the production of finely graded forces. , 2005, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[4]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Alternating prism exposure causes dual adaptation and generalization to a novel displacement , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.

[5]  I. Kohler,et al.  The formation and transformation of the perceptual world. , 1963 .

[6]  A. McIntosh,et al.  Time course of changes in brain activity and functional connectivity associated with long-term adaptation to a rotational transformation. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  Claus Svarer,et al.  Cluster analysis of activity‐time series in motor learning , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[8]  G. M. Redding,et al.  Adaptive spatial alignment and strategic perceptual-motor control. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  R. Daroff,et al.  Clinical Neurology for Psychiatrists , 1995, Neurology.

[10]  E. Taub,et al.  Use of Sensory Recombination and Somatosensory Deafferentation Techniques in the Investigation of Sensory-Motor Integration , 1974, Perception.

[11]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Conditions for interference versus facilitation during sequential sensorimotor adaptation , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[12]  S. Kinomura,et al.  Activity in the parietal area during visuomotor learning with optical rotation , 1997, Neuroreport.

[13]  Ferdinando A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Interaction of visual and proprioceptive feedback during adaptation of human reaching movements. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  J P Roll,et al.  Inhibitory effects of combined agonist and antagonist muscle vibration on H-reflex in man. , 1986, Aviation, space, and environmental medicine.

[15]  D. Willingham,et al.  Deficit in learning of a motor skill requiring strategy, but not of perceptuomotor recalibration, with aging. , 1998, Learning & memory.

[16]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  N Teasdale,et al.  Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient and normal subjects , 1992, Neurology.

[18]  J. Lackner,et al.  Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Otmar Bock,et al.  Sensorimotor adaptation to visual distortions with different kinematic coupling , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  G. M. Gauthier,et al.  Weight estimation in a "deafferented" man and in control subjects: are judgements influenced by peripheral or central signals? , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Nicolas Vuillerme,et al.  Postural sway under muscle vibration and muscle fatigue in humans , 2002, Neuroscience Letters.

[22]  M. Hallett,et al.  Adaptation to lateral displacement of vision in patients with lesions of the central nervous system , 1983, Neurology.

[23]  B. Stein,et al.  The Merging of the Senses , 1993 .

[24]  J. C. Gilhodes,et al.  Perceptual and motor effects of agonist-antagonist muscle vibration in man , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[25]  R. Miall,et al.  The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  J. Roll,et al.  Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[27]  Christophe Bourdin,et al.  Role of sensory information in updating internal models of the effector during arm tracking. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[28]  R L Sainburg,et al.  Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  C. Bard,et al.  Contribution of proprioception for calibrating and updating the motor space. , 1995, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[30]  H. Pick,et al.  Visual capture produced by prism spectacles , 1965 .

[31]  John W. Krakauer,et al.  Independent learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of reaching , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  C. Spence,et al.  Multisensory integration and the body schema: close to hand and within reach , 2003, Current Biology.

[33]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[34]  J Blouin,et al.  Adaptation in visuomanual tracking depends on intact proprioception. , 1998, Journal of motor behavior.