Transfer in Rule-Based Category Learning Depends on the Training Task

While learning is often highly specific to the exact stimuli and tasks used during training, there are cases where training results in learning that generalizes more broadly. It has been previously argued that the degree of specificity can be predicted based upon the learning solution(s) dictated by the particular demands of the training task. Here we applied this logic in the domain of rule-based categorization learning. Participants were presented with stimuli corresponding to four different categories and were asked to perform either a category discrimination task (which permits learning specific rule to discriminate two categories) or a category identification task (which does not permit learning a specific discrimination rule). In a subsequent transfer stage, all participants were asked to discriminate stimuli belonging to two of the categories which they had seen, but had never directly discriminated before (i.e., this particular discrimination was omitted from training). As predicted, learning in the category-discrimination tasks tended to be specific, while the category-identification task produced learning that transferred to the transfer discrimination task. These results suggest that the discrimination and identification tasks fostered the acquisition of different category representations which were more or less generalizable.

[1]  C. S. Green,et al.  Orientation Transfer in Vernier and Stereoacuity Training , 2015, PloS one.

[2]  Florian Kattner,et al.  Differences in perceptual learning transfer as a function of training task. , 2015, Journal of vision.

[3]  Kimery R. Levering,et al.  Observation versus classification in supervised category learning , 2015, Memory & cognition.

[4]  S. Klein,et al.  Vernier perceptual learning transfers to completely untrained retinal locations after double training: a "piggybacking" effect. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[5]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Applying perceptual learning to achieve practical changes in vision , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[6]  Aaron R. Seitz,et al.  Improved vision and on-field performance in baseball through perceptual learning , 2014, Current Biology.

[7]  J. D. Smith,et al.  Deferred Feedback Sharply Dissociates Implicit and Explicit Category Learning , 2014, Psychological science.

[8]  Paul R. Schrater,et al.  Task-Specific Response Strategy Selection on the Basis of Recent Training Experience , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..

[9]  Robert M Nosofsky,et al.  Category number impacts rule-based and information-integration category learning: a reassessment of evidence for dissociable category-learning systems. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[10]  Monica Melby-Lervåg,et al.  Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[11]  C. S. Green,et al.  Brain plasticity through the life span: learning to learn and action video games. , 2012, Annual review of neuroscience.

[12]  S. Klein,et al.  Task relevancy and demand modulate double-training enabled transfer of perceptual learning , 2012, Vision Research.

[13]  S. Hélie,et al.  Learning and transfer of category knowledge in an indirect categorization task , 2011, Psychological Research.

[14]  C. S. Green,et al.  Learning, Attentional Control, and Action Video Games , 2012, Current Biology.

[15]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayes factor approaches for testing interval null hypotheses. , 2011, Psychological methods.

[16]  Brian H. Ross,et al.  Classification versus inference learning contrasted with real-world categories , 2011, Memory & cognition.

[17]  D. Sagi Perceptual learning in Vision Research , 2011, Vision Research.

[18]  Yasuaki Sakamoto,et al.  Learning and retention through predictive inference and classification. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Applied.

[19]  Aaron B. Hoffman,et al.  The costs of supervised classification: The effect of learning task on conceptual flexibility. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[20]  Jessica A. Grahn,et al.  Putting brain training to the test , 2010, Nature.

[21]  M. Fahle,et al.  Orientation specificity of learning vernier discriminations , 2010, Vision Research.

[22]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Decoupling location specificity from perceptual learning of orientation discrimination , 2010, Vision Research.

[23]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis , 2009, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[24]  S. Klein,et al.  Complete Transfer of Perceptual Learning across Retinal Locations Enabled by Double Training , 2008, Current Biology.

[25]  D. Bavelier,et al.  Exercising your brain: a review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. , 2008, Psychology and aging.

[26]  R. Bjork,et al.  Learning Concepts and Categories , 2008, Psychological science.

[27]  David J Ostry,et al.  Specificity of Speech Motor Learning , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  C S Green,et al.  Action-Video-Game Experience Alters the Spatial Resolution of Vision , 2007, Psychological science.

[29]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Theory-based Bayesian models of inductive learning and reasoning , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  John K Kruschke,et al.  Category representation for classification and feature inference. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[31]  M. Fahle Perceptual learning: specificity versus generalization , 2005, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[32]  B. Ross,et al.  Inference and classification learning of abstract coherent categories. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[33]  David L. Faigman,et al.  Human category learning. , 2005, Annual review of psychology.

[34]  M. Fahle Perceptual learning: a case for early selection. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[35]  S. Hochstein,et al.  The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[36]  John R Anderson,et al.  An integrated theory of the mind. , 2004, Psychological review.

[37]  W. T. Maddox,et al.  Dissociating explicit and procedural-learning based systems of perceptual category learning , 2004, Behavioural Processes.

[38]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Semantic Cognition: A Parallel Distributed Processing Approach , 2004 .

[39]  D. Medin,et al.  SUSTAIN: a network model of category learning. , 2004, Psychological review.

[40]  Seth Chin-Parker,et al.  Diagnosticity and prototypicality in category learning: a comparison of inference learning and classification learning. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[41]  W Todd Maddox,et al.  Category number impacts rule-based but not information-integration category learning: further evidence for dissociable category-learning systems. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[42]  A. Markman,et al.  Category use and category learning. , 2003, Psychological bulletin.

[43]  Nestor Matthews,et al.  Task-specific perceptual learning on speed and direction discrimination , 2003, Vision Research.

[44]  A. Markman,et al.  Learning nonlinearly separable categories by inference and classification. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[45]  Seth Chin-Parker,et al.  The effect of category learning on sensitivity to within-category correlations , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[46]  Seth Chin-Parker,et al.  A further investigation of category learning by inference , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[47]  Susan M. Barnett,et al.  When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[48]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. , 1998, Psychological review.

[49]  A. Markman,et al.  Category Learning by Inference and Classification , 1998 .

[50]  Robert L. Goldstone,et al.  Reuniting perception and conception , 1998, Cognition.

[51]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning , 1997, Nature.

[52]  F. Rauscher,et al.  Music training causes long-term enhancement of preschool children's spatial-temporal reasoning. , 1997, Neurological research.

[53]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[54]  D G Pelli,et al.  Pixel independence: measuring spatial interactions on a CRT display. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[55]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[56]  Gordon D. Logan,et al.  ATTENTION IN THE ACQUISITION AND EXPRESSION OF AUTOMATICITY , 1996 .

[57]  G. Logan,et al.  "What is learned during automatization? The role of attention in constructing an instance": Correction to Logan and Etherton. , 1994 .

[58]  R. Nosofsky,et al.  Rule-plus-exception model of classification learning. , 1994, Psychological review.

[59]  M. Jitsumori Category discrimination of artificial polymorphous stimuli based on feature learning. , 1993 .

[60]  R. Schmidt,et al.  New Conceptualizations of Practice: Common Principles in Three Paradigms Suggest New Concepts for Training , 1992 .

[61]  John R. Anderson,et al.  The Adaptive Nature of Human Categorization , 1991 .

[62]  Gregory Ashby,et al.  Decision rules in the perception and categorization of multidimensional stimuli. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[63]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[64]  D. Medin,et al.  Family resemblance, conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.

[65]  R. Magill,et al.  Contextual Interference Effects in Learning Three Badminton Serves , 1986 .

[66]  R. Sekuler,et al.  A specific and enduring improvement in visual motion discrimination. , 1982, Science.

[67]  A. Fiorentini,et al.  Perceptual learning specific for orientation and spatial frequency , 1980, Nature.