Widespread access to predictive models in the motor system: a short review

Recent behavioural and computational studies suggest that access to internal predictive models of arm and object dynamics is widespread in the sensorimotor system. Several systems, including those responsible for oculomotor and skeletomotor control, perceptual processing, postural control and mental imagery, are able to access predictions of the motion of the arm. A capacity to make and use predictions of object dynamics is similarly widespread. Here, we review recent studies looking at the predictive capacity of the central nervous system which reveal pervasive access to forward models of the environment.

[1]  E. Holst Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs , 1954 .

[2]  L. Weiskrantz,et al.  Preliminary Observations on Tickling Oneself , 1971, Nature.

[3]  C. Marsden,et al.  Anticipatory postural responses in the human subject [proceedings]. , 1978, The Journal of physiology.

[4]  S. Bouisset,et al.  A sequence of postural movements precedes voluntary movement , 1981, Neuroscience Letters.

[5]  C. Atkeson,et al.  Learning arm kinematics and dynamics. , 1989, Annual review of neuroscience.

[6]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Forward Models: Supervised Learning with a Distal Teacher , 1992, Cogn. Sci..

[7]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensory-motor coordination during grasping and manipulative actions , 1992, Current Biology.

[8]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[10]  Hans Forssberg,et al.  Formation and lateralization of internal representations underlying motor commands during precision grip , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[11]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  An internal model for sensorimotor integration. , 1995, Science.

[12]  Chi-Sang Poon,et al.  Self-tuning Optimal Regulation of Respiratory Motor Output by Hebbian Covariance Learning , 1996, Neural Networks.

[13]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control , 1996, Neural Networks.

[14]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[15]  F. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[17]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Spatio-Temporal Prediction Modulates the Perception of Self-Produced Stimuli , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  I Salimi,et al.  Specificity of internal representations underlying grasping. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  Jefferson E. Roy,et al.  Selective Processing of Vestibular Reafference during Self-Generated Head Motion , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  J. Flanagan,et al.  The Inertial Anisotropy of the Arm Is Accurately Predicted during Movement Planning , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  Mitsuo Kawato,et al.  MOSAIC Model for Sensorimotor Learning and Control , 2001, Neural Computation.

[22]  Martin Faint,et al.  Does the brain model newton’s laws? , 2001 .

[23]  Olivier White,et al.  The effects of a change in gravity on the dynamics of prehension , 2002, Experimental Brain Research.

[24]  Amir Karniel,et al.  Three creatures named 'forward model' , 2002, Neural Networks.

[25]  R. Shadmehr,et al.  A Real-Time State Predictor in Motor Control: Study of Saccadic Eye Movements during Unseen Reaching Movements , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Christian Darlot,et al.  Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements , 2002, Biological Cybernetics.

[27]  Stefan Schaal,et al.  Forward models in visuomotor control. , 2002, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  R. Wurtz,et al.  A Pathway in Primate Brain for Internal Monitoring of Movements , 2002, Science.

[29]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Sensorimotor Memory For Fingertip Forces: Evidence For A Task-Independent Motor Memory , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[30]  M. Kawato,et al.  Modular organization of internal models of tools in the human cerebellum , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[31]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Evidence for a specific internal representation of motion–force relationships during object manipulation , 2003, Biological Cybernetics.

[32]  Thrishantha Nanayakkara,et al.  Saccade adaptation in response to altered arm dynamics. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Sensorimotor memory and grip force control: does grip force anticipate a self‐produced weight change when drinking with a straw from a cup? , 2003, The European journal of neuroscience.

[34]  M. Kawato,et al.  Internal forward models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. , 2003, Progress in brain research.

[35]  Joachim Hermsdörfer,et al.  Memory for fingertip forces: passive hand muscle vibration interferes with predictive grip force scaling , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[36]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Cerebellar Contribution to Saccades and Gaze Holding , 2003 .

[37]  Daniel M. Wolpert,et al.  Internal models underlying grasp can be additively combined , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  S. Glasauer,et al.  Cerebellar contribution to saccades and gaze holding: a modeling approach. , 2003, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[39]  A. Gordon,et al.  Internal representations underlying respiration during object manipulation , 2003, Brain Research.

[40]  R. Johansson,et al.  Prediction Precedes Control in Motor Learning , 2003, Current Biology.

[41]  D. Wolpert,et al.  Two Eyes for an Eye: The Neuroscience of Force Escalation , 2003, Science.

[42]  R. Johansson,et al.  Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  R. Gentili,et al.  Inertial properties of the arm are accurately predicted during motor imagery , 2004, Behavioural Brain Research.

[45]  E. Holst,et al.  Das Reafferenzprinzip , 2004, Naturwissenschaften.

[46]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  Moving objects in a rotating environment: rapid prediction of Coriolis and centrifugal force perturbations , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[47]  R. Johansson,et al.  Visual size cues in the programming of manipulative forces during precision grip , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[48]  Stefan Glasauer,et al.  How predictive is grip force control in the complete absence of somatosensory feedback? , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[49]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Fast adaptation of the internal model of gravity for manual interceptions: evidence for event-dependent learning. , 2005, Journal of neurophysiology.