Modelling Prosaccade Latencies across Multiple Decision-Making Tasks

Oculomotor decision making can be investigated by a simple step task, where a person decides whether a target has jumped to the left or the right. More complex tasks include the countermanding task (look at the jumped target, except when a subsequent signal instructs you not to) and the Wheeless task (where the jumped target sometimes then quickly jumps to a new location). Different instantiations of the LATER (Linear Approach to Threshold with Ergodic Rate) model have been shown to explain the saccadic latency data arising from these tasks, despite it being almost inconceivable that completely separate decision-making mechanisms exist for each. However, these models have an identical construction with regards to predicting prosaccadic responses (all step task trials, and control trials in countermanding and Wheeless tasks, where no countermanding signal is given or when the target does not make a second jump). We measured saccadic latencies for 23 human observers each performing the three tasks, and modelled prosaccade latencies with LATER to see if model parameters were usefully preserved across tasks. We found no significant difference in reaction times and model parameters between the step and Wheeless tasks (mean175 & 177ms, respectively; standard deviation, SD 22 & 24ms). In contrast, we identified prolonged latencies in the countermanding tasks (236ms; SD 37ms) explained by a slower rise and an elevated threshold of the decision making signal, suggesting elevated participant caution. Our findings support the idea that common machinery exists for oculomotor decision-making, which can be flexibly deployed depending upon task demands.

[1]  J. Mollon,et al.  Luminance noise and the rapid determination of discrimination ellipses in colour deficiency , 1994, Vision Research.

[2]  T. Schweizer,et al.  Saccadic Eye Movements in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: A Pilot Study OPEN ACCESS , 2014, Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences / Journal Canadien des Sciences Neurologiques.

[3]  G. D. Logan,et al.  Dynamics of saccade target selection: Race model analysis of double step and search step saccade production in human and macaque , 2007, Vision Research.

[4]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Antisaccades as decisions: LATER model predicts latency distributions and error responses , 2013, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  J. Schall,et al.  Countermanding saccades in macaque , 1995, Visual Neuroscience.

[6]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Huntington’s disease: changes in saccades and hand-tapping over 3 years , 2010, Journal of Neurology.

[7]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Directional Prediction by the Saccadic System , 2008, Current Biology.

[8]  Q. Gong,et al.  Differing proportions of ‘express saccade makers’ in different human populations , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[9]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The Effect of Cognitive Distraction on Saccadic Latency , 2010, Perception.

[10]  Árni Kristjánsson,et al.  Less attention is more in the preparation of antisaccades, but not prosaccades , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  E. Salinas,et al.  The Countermanding Task Revisited: Fast Stimulus Detection Is a Key Determinant of Psychophysical Performance , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Movements of the Eyes , 1978 .

[13]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[14]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Saccadic countermanding: a comparison of central and peripheral stop signals , 2001, Vision Research.

[15]  A. J. Anderson,et al.  Contrast discrimination under task-induced mental load , 2019, Vision Research.

[16]  Gordon D Logan,et al.  Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. , 2003, Acta psychologica.

[17]  T. Braver,et al.  A theory of cognitive control, aging cognition, and neuromodulation , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[18]  L L Wheeless,et al.  Eye-movement responses to step and pulse-step stimuli. , 1966, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[19]  R. H. S. Carpenter,et al.  Neural computation of log likelihood in control of saccadic eye movements , 1995, Nature.

[20]  R H S Carpenter,et al.  Ultrafast initiation of a neural race by impending errors , 2015, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  J. Stahl,et al.  Amplitude of human head movements associated with horizontal saccades , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[22]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[23]  B A J Reddi,et al.  Accuracy, information, and response time in a saccadic decision task. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  N. Smyrnis,et al.  Larger variability of saccadic reaction times in schizophrenia patients , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[25]  Russell J. Boag,et al.  Strategic attention and decision control support prospective memory in a complex dual-task environment , 2019, Cognition.

[26]  Chrystalina A Antoniades,et al.  The relationship between abnormalities of saccadic and manual response times in Parkinson's disease. , 2013, Journal of Parkinson's disease.

[27]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[28]  Jonathon Love,et al.  A Bayesian approach for estimating the probability of trigger failures in the stop-signal paradigm , 2016, Behavior research methods.

[29]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Countermanding saccades in humans , 1999, Vision Research.

[30]  Imran Noorani,et al.  Re‐starting a neural race: anti‐saccade correction , 2014, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Saccadic latency distributions in Parkinson’s disease and the effects of l-dopa , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  Imran Noorani,et al.  Towards a unifying mechanism for cancelling movements , 2017, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[33]  M. Landy,et al.  A Two-Stage Process Model of Sensory Discrimination: An Alternative to Drift-Diffusion , 2016, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[34]  Scott D. Brown,et al.  The simplest complete model of choice response time: Linear ballistic accumulation , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[35]  R. Carpenter,et al.  The influence of urgency on decision time , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  N. Smyrnis,et al.  Variability in the decision process leading to saccades: a specific marker for schizophrenia? , 2014, Psychophysiology.

[37]  R. Carpenter,et al.  Contrast, Probability, and Saccadic Latency Evidence for Independence of Detection and Decision , 2004, Current Biology.