Modeling the integration of motion signals across space.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Roger B. H. Tootell,et al. Segregation of global and local motion processing in primate middle temporal visual area , 1992, Nature.
[2] M. Shiffrar,et al. Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.
[3] Christopher C. Pack,et al. The role of terminators and occlusion cues in motion integration and segmentation: a neural network model , 1999, Vision Research.
[4] D. Field,et al. Integration of contours: new insights , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[5] A. Grinvald,et al. Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[6] J Lorenceau,et al. Cooperative and competitive spatial interactions in motion integration , 1999, Visual Neuroscience.
[7] J. Cowan,et al. A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue , 1973, Kybernetik.
[8] R. Sekuler,et al. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.
[9] Christopher C. Pack,et al. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.
[10] T. Albright. Direction and orientation selectivity of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. , 1984, Journal of neurophysiology.
[11] U. Polat,et al. Lateral interactions between spatial channels: Suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments , 1993, Vision Research.
[12] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Direction repulsion between components in motion transparency , 1996, Vision Research.
[13] K. Nakayama,et al. The aperture problem—I. Perception of nonrigidity and motion direction in translating sinusoidal lines , 1988, Vision Research.
[14] Shaul Hochstein,et al. Isolating the effect of one-dimensional motion signals on the perceived direction of moving two-dimensional objects , 1993, Vision Research.
[15] H. Wilson. A model for direction selectivity in threshold motion perception , 2004, Biological Cybernetics.
[16] S. Dakin,et al. Snakes and ladders: the role of temporal modulation in visual contour integration , 2001, Vision Research.
[17] Maggie Shiffrar,et al. The influence of terminators on motion integration across space , 1992, Vision Research.
[18] Anthony J. Movshon,et al. Visual Response Properties of Striate Cortical Neurons Projecting to Area MT in Macaque Monkeys , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[19] James T. Todd,et al. The perception of globally coherent motion , 1992, Vision Research.
[20] Mercedes Barchilon Ben-Av,et al. Disambiguating velocity estimates across image space , 1995, Vision Research.
[21] Jeffrey S. Perry,et al. Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance , 2001, Vision Research.
[22] Robert F. Hess,et al. Dynamics of contour integration , 2001, Vision Research.
[23] Hugh R. Wilson,et al. Hysteresis in binocular grating perception: Contrast effects , 1977, Vision Research.
[24] S. Wuerger,et al. Perception of Moving Lines: Interactions Between Local Perpendicular Signals and 2D Motion Signals , 1997, Vision Research.
[25] Gunter Loffler. The integration of motion signals across space. , 1999 .
[26] H. Wilson,et al. A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.
[27] D. G. Albrecht,et al. Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] W Singer,et al. The Perceptual Grouping Criterion of Colinearity is Reflected by Anisotropies of Connections in the Primary Visual Cortex , 1997, The European journal of neuroscience.
[29] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[30] S. Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of motion perception: Direction fields, apertures, and resonant grouping , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] Stephen Grossberg,et al. Neural dynamics of motion grouping: from aperture ambiguity to object speed and direction , 1997 .
[32] Hans Wallach. Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung , 1935 .
[33] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability , 1984, Vision Research.
[34] H. Wilson,et al. Motion Integration over Space: Interaction of the Center and Surround Motion* * This research was first reported at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, May 1994 and 1995. , 1997, Vision Research.
[35] W. Reichardt,et al. Autocorrelation, a principle for the evaluation of sensory information by the central nervous system , 1961 .
[36] John H. R. Maunsell,et al. Coding of image contrast in central visual pathways of the macaque monkey , 1990, Vision Research.
[37] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of local and wide-field movements in the superior temporal visual areas of the macaque monkey , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[39] H. Wilson,et al. Modified line-element theory for spatial-frequency and width discrimination. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[40] B Moulden,et al. A Simultaneous Shift in Apparent Direction: Further Evidence for a “Distribution-Shift” Model of Direction Coding , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[41] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[42] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Perceived motion in the vector sum direction , 1994, Vision Research.
[43] G. Orban,et al. Shape and Spatial Distribution of Receptive Fields and Antagonistic Motion Surrounds in the Middle Temporal Area (V5) of the Macaque , 1995, The European journal of neuroscience.
[44] R. Gregory. Stars to Eyes by Adaptive Optics , 2002, Perception.
[45] F. Kooi. Local direction of edge motion causes and abolishes the barberpole illusion , 1993, Vision Research.
[46] J. Movshon,et al. The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[47] H R Wilson,et al. Curvature and separation discrimination at texture boundaries. , 1992, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[48] K. Nakayama,et al. Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion , 1989, Vision Research.
[49] Ennio Mingolla,et al. Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon , 1998, Vision Research.
[50] J. B. Mulligan,et al. Effect of contrast on the perceived direction of a moving plaid , 1990, Vision Research.
[51] W. Newsome,et al. Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[52] D C Van Essen,et al. Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.
[53] Harry S. Orbach,et al. Computing feature motion without feature detectors: A model for terminator motion without end-stopped cells 1 This research was first reported at the Annual Meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, May 1997. 1 , 1999, Vision Research.
[54] T. Sejnowski,et al. A selection model for motion processing in area MT of primates , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[55] Eric Castet,et al. The extrinsic/intrinsic classification of two-dimensional motion signals with barber-pole stimuli , 1999, Vision Research.
[56] David Bradley. MT signals: better with time , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[57] R. Sekuler,et al. Assimilation and contrast in motion perception: Explorations in cooperativity , 1990, Vision Research.
[58] H R Wilson,et al. A model for motion coherence and transparency , 1994, Visual Neuroscience.
[59] J. Lund,et al. Widespread periodic intrinsic connections in the tree shrew visual cortex. , 1982, Science.
[60] G. Blasdel,et al. Physiological organization of layer 4 in macaque striate cortex , 1984, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[61] J. Allman,et al. Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. , 1985, Annual review of neuroscience.
[62] C D Salzman,et al. Neural mechanisms for forming a perceptual decision. , 1994, Science.
[63] Gunter Loffler,et al. Factors affecting motion integration. , 2003, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[64] D Purves,et al. A wholly empirical explanation of perceived motion , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[65] H. Rodman,et al. Single-unit analysis of pattern-motion selective properties in the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[66] A. Georgopoulos,et al. Cognitive neurophysiology of the motor cortex. , 1993, Science.
[67] D. Fitzpatrick,et al. Orientation Selectivity and the Arrangement of Horizontal Connections in Tree Shrew Striate Cortex , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[68] Jeounghoon Kim,et al. Dependence of plaid motion coherence on component grating directions , 1993, Vision Research.
[69] Richard A. Andersen,et al. Coordinate transformations in the representation of spatial information , 1993, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[70] Hugh R. Wilson,et al. Perceived direction of moving two-dimensional patterns depends on duration, contrast and eccentricity , 1992, Vision Research.
[71] Mark A. Georgeson,et al. The primal sketch revisited: locating and representing edges in human vision via Gaussian-derivative filtering , 2002 .
[72] Michael S. Landy,et al. Theories for the Visual Perception of Local Velocity and Coherent Motion , 1991 .
[73] Robert Jefferson Snowden,et al. Motions in orthogonal directions are mutually suppressive , 1989 .
[74] J. van Santen,et al. Elaborated Reichardt detectors. , 1985, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.