Neural dynamics of adaptive timing and temporal discrimination during associative learning

Abstract A neural network model that controls behavioral timing is described and simulated. This model, called the Spectral Timing Model, controls a type of timing whereby an animal or robot can learn to wait for an expected goal by discounting expected nonoccurrences of a goal object until the expected time of arrival of the goal. If the goal object does not then materialize, the animal can respond to unexpected nonoccurrences of the goal with appropriate changes in information processing and exploratory behavior. The model is a variant of the gated dipole model of opponent processing. When the gated dipole model is generalized to include a spectrum of cellular response rates within a large population of cells, the model's total output signal generates accurate learned timing properties that collectively provide a good quantitative fit to animal learning data. In particular, the Spectral Timing Model utilizes the habituative transmitter gates and adaptive long-term memory traces that are characteristic of gated dipole models. The Spectral Timing Model is embedded into an Adaptive Resonance Theory (ART) neural architecture for the learning of correlations between internal representations of recognition codes and reinforcement codes. This type of learning is called conditioned reinforcer learning. The two types of internal representations are called sensory representations (S) and drive representations (D). Activation of a drive representation D by the Spectral Timing Model inhibits output signals from the orienting subsystem (A) of the ART architecture and activates a motor response. The inhibitory pathway helps to prevent spurious resets of short-term memory, forgetting, and orienting responses from being caused by events other than the goal object prior to the expected arrival time of the goal. Simulated data properties include the inverted U in learning as a function of the interstimulus interval (ISI) that occurs between onset of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US); correlations of peak time, standard deviation, Weber fraction, and peak amplitude of the conditioned response as a function of the ISI; increase of conditioned response amplitude, but not its timing, with US intensity; speed-up of the timing circuit by an increase in CS intensity or by drugs that increase concentrations of brain dopamine or acetylcholine; multiple timing peaks in response to learning conditions using multiple ISIs; and conditioned timing of cell activation within the hippocampus and of the contingent negative variation (CNV) event-related potential. The results on speed-up by drugs that increase brain concentrations of dopamine and acetylcholine support a 1972 prediction that the gated dipole habituative transmitter is a catecholamine and its long-term memory trace transmitter is acetylcholine. It is noted that the timing circuit described herein is only one of several functionally distinct neural circuits for governing different types of timed behavior competence.

[1]  M. C. Smith,et al.  CS-US interval and US intensity in classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response. , 1968, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[2]  D M Parker,et al.  The Spatial Selectivity of Early and Late Waves within the Human Visual Evoked Response , 1977, Perception.

[3]  G. Plant,et al.  Transient visually evoked potentials to the pattern reversal and onset of sinusoidal gratings. , 1983, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[4]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit's classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. , 1986, Behavioral neuroscience.

[5]  W. Skrandies Scalp potential fields evoked by grating stimuli: effects of spatial frequency and orientation. , 1984, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[6]  P. Solomon Temporal versus spatial information processing theories of hippocampal function. , 1979, Psychological bulletin.

[7]  D. Mcadam Increases in CNS excitability during negative cortical slow potentials in man. , 1969, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[8]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of speech and language coding: developmental programs, perceptual grouping, and competition for short-term memory. , 1986, Human neurobiology.

[9]  D. Robinson Oculomotor unit behavior in the monkey. , 1970, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  M. Low,et al.  Surface‐negative, slow‐potential shift associated with conditioning in man , 1966, Neurology.

[11]  J R Lishman,et al.  The early wave of the visual evoked potential to sinusoidal gratings: responses to quadrant stimulation as a function of spatial frequency. , 1982, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[12]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Activity of brain stem neurons during eye movements of alert monkeys. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[13]  J R Lishman,et al.  Visual-evoked responses elicited by the onset and offset of sinusoidal gratings: latency, waveform, and topographic characteristics. , 1982, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[14]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A massively parallel architecture for a self-organizing neural pattern recognition machine , 1988, Comput. Vis. Graph. Image Process..

[15]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of attentionally modulated Pavlovian conditioning: blocking, interstimulus interval, and secondary reinforcement. , 1987, Applied optics.

[16]  A. Vassilev,et al.  On the latency of human visually evoked response to sinusoidal gratings , 1979, Vision Research.

[17]  D. Wilkie Stimulus intensity affects pigeons’ timing behavior: Implications for an internal clock model , 1987 .

[18]  P. Killeen,et al.  Optimal timing and the Weber function. , 1987, Psychological review.

[19]  J. Konorski Conditioned reflexes and neuron organization. , 1948 .

[20]  W. Meck Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[21]  R. Church,et al.  Methamphetamine and time estimation. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[22]  E. Henneman Relation between size of neurons and their susceptibility to discharge. , 1957, Science.

[23]  D. Jeffreys,et al.  The influence of spatial frequency on the reaction times and evoked potentials recorded to grating pattern stimuli , 1985, Vision Research.

[24]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Latency of the neuromagnetic response of the human visual cortex , 1978, Vision Research.

[25]  S Grossberg,et al.  On the dynamics of operant conditioning. , 1971, Journal of theoretical biology.

[26]  J. Rawlins,et al.  Associations across time: The hippocampus as a temporary memory store , 1985, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of word recognition and recall: attentional priming, learning, and resonance. , 1986 .

[28]  Richard F. Thompson,et al.  Neuronal plasticity in the limbic system during classical conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response. I. The hippocampus , 1978, Brain Research.

[29]  S. Grossberg Neural Networks and Natural Intelligence , 1988 .

[30]  R. Boice,et al.  The conditioned licking response in rats as a function of the CS-UCS interval , 1965 .

[31]  P. Solomon A time and a place for everything? Temporal processing views of hippocampal function with special reference to attention , 1980 .

[32]  S Grossberg,et al.  Masking fields: a massively parallel neural architecture for learning, recognizing, and predicting multiple groupings of patterned data. , 1987, Applied optics.

[33]  A. Vassilev,et al.  Spatial frequency and the pattern onset-offset response , 1983, Vision Research.

[34]  D. Mcadam,et al.  Classical conditioning in the cat as a function of the CS-US interval , 1965 .

[35]  R. Church,et al.  Cholinergic modulation of the content of temporal memory. , 1987, Behavioral neuroscience.

[36]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Neural Theory of Punishment and Avoidance, II: Quantitative Theory , 1972 .

[37]  D. M. Parker,et al.  Latency changes in the human visual evoked response to sinusoidal gratings , 1977, Vision Research.

[38]  R. Church,et al.  Nucleus basalis magnocellularis and medial septal area lesions differentially impair temporal memory , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[39]  Liberson Cw,et al.  Eye movements during the alert state (psychophysiological correlations under resting conditions). , 1967 .

[40]  S. Grossberg A neural model of attention, reinforcement and discrimination learning. , 1975, International review of neurobiology.

[41]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of adaptive sensory-motor control : ballistic eye movements , 1986 .

[42]  E. Henneman The size-principle: a deterministic output emerges from a set of probabilistic connections. , 1985, The Journal of experimental biology.

[43]  J. Staddon Adaptive behavior and learning , 1983 .

[44]  R Jones,et al.  Visual evoked response as a function of grating spatial frequency. , 1978, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[45]  J. R. Millenson,et al.  Classical conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response under fixed and mixed CS-US intervals , 1977 .

[46]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Effect of the interstimulus (CS-UCS) interval on hippocampal unit activity during classical conditioning of the nictitating membrane response of the rabbit (Oryctolagus cuniculus). , 1980, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[47]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .

[48]  J. Delacour,et al.  Conditioning to time: Evidence for a role of hippocampus from unit recording , 1987, Neuroscience.

[49]  R. Church,et al.  Simultaneous temporal processing. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[50]  R. Black,et al.  Heart rate conditioning as a function of interstimulus interval in rats , 1967 .

[51]  S. Grossberg,et al.  Neural dynamics of attentionally modulated Pavlovian conditioning: Conditioned reinforcement, inhibition, and opponent processing , 1987, Psychobiology.

[52]  S. Grossberg Studies of mind and brain : neural principles of learning, perception, development, cognition, and motor control , 1982 .

[53]  J. Ayres,et al.  CS and US duration effects in one-trial simultaneous fear conditioning as assessed by conditioned suppression of licking in rats , 1978 .

[54]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  The ART of adaptive pattern recognition by a self-organizing neural network , 1987, Computer.