Dissociation between the Perceptual and Saccadic Localization of Moving Objects

Visual processing in the human brain provides the data both for perception and for guiding motor actions. It seems natural that our actions would be directed toward perceived locations of their targets, but it has been proposed that action and perception rely on different visual information [1-4], and this provocative claim has triggered a long-lasting debate [5-7]. Here, in support of this claim, we report a large, robust dissociation between perception and action. We take advantage of a perceptual illusion in which visual motion signals presented within the boundaries of a peripheral moving object can make the object's apparent trajectory deviate by 45° or more from its physical trajectory [8-10], a shift several times larger than the typical discrimination threshold for motion direction [11]. Despite the large perceptual distortion, we found that saccadic eye movements directed to these moving objects clearly targeted locations along their physical rather than apparent trajectories. We show that the perceived trajectory is based on the accumulation of position error determined by prior sensory history-an accumulation of error that is not found for the action toward the same target. We suggest that visual processing for perception and action might diverge in how past information is combined with new visual input, with action relying only on immediate information to track a target, whereas perception builds on previous estimates to construct a conscious representation.

[1]  M. Donk,et al.  Awareness of the saccade goal in oculomotor selection: Your eyes go before you know , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.

[2]  R. Gellman,et al.  Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[3]  David A. Robinson,et al.  Models of the saccadic eye movement control system , 1973, Kybernetik.

[4]  A Mack,et al.  Saccadic programming and perceived location. , 1981, Acta psychologica.

[5]  Masahiro Takei,et al.  Human resource development and visualization , 2009, J. Vis..

[6]  M. Morgan,et al.  Rapid eye movements to a virtual target are biased by illusory context in the Poggendorff figure , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[7]  J. McCarley,et al.  State-trace analysis of the effects of a visual illusion on saccade amplitudes and perceptual judgments , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  David C. Burr,et al.  Separate visual representations for perception and action revealed by saccadic eye movements , 2001, Current Biology.

[9]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  The flash grab effect , 2012, Vision Research.

[10]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Tracking Without Perceiving , 2011, Psychology Science.

[11]  Béatrice de Gelder,et al.  Collicular Vision Guides Nonconscious Behavior , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[12]  P. Sterzer,et al.  A direct oculomotor correlate of unconscious visual processing , 2012, Current Biology.

[13]  David Whitney,et al.  Visual motion shifts saccade targets , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.

[14]  R. McIntosh,et al.  Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.

[15]  M. Goodale,et al.  Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.

[16]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.

[17]  S. Mateeff,et al.  Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.

[18]  H. Deubel ADAPTIVITY OF GAIN AND DIRECTION IN OBLIQUE SACCADES1 , 1987 .

[19]  Tirin Moore,et al.  Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2007, Neuron.

[20]  H. Ono,et al.  Saccadic eye movements during changes in fixation to stimuli at different distances , 1977, Vision Research.

[21]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Unifying account of visual motion and position perception , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  A Straube,et al.  Differential effect of a distractor on primary saccades and perceptual localization , 2002, Vision Research.

[23]  Alan Cowey,et al.  On the usefulness of ‘what’ and ‘where’ pathways in vision , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  J. Movshon,et al.  Integration of sensory evidence in motion discrimination. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[25]  J. V. Gisbergen,et al.  Scatter in the metrics of saccades and properties of the collicular motor map , 1989, Vision Research.

[26]  Current Biology , 2012, Current Biology.

[27]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Masking produces compression of space and time in the absence of eye movements. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[29]  Leslie Welch,et al.  The precision of size constancy , 1992, Vision Research.

[30]  Chang-Bing Huang,et al.  Transitions between Central and Peripheral Vision Create Spatial/Temporal Distortions: A Hypothesis Concerning the Perceived Break of the Curveball , 2010, PloS one.

[31]  David Whitney,et al.  The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[32]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[33]  Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al.  Contrast and assimilation in motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[35]  David Whitney,et al.  The Emergence of Perceived Position in the Visual System , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[36]  Nicola Bruno,et al.  When does action resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[37]  Dirk Kerzel,et al.  Motion-induced illusory displacement reexamined: differences between perception and action? , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  Maria Concetta Morrone,et al.  Visual motion distorts visual and motor space. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[39]  Leland S. Stone,et al.  Expansion of Direction Space around the Cardinal Axes Revealed by Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2005, Neuron.

[40]  P.-J. Hsieh,et al.  The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals , 2006, Vision Research.

[41]  David C. Burr,et al.  Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.

[42]  T. Schenk,et al.  The Premotor theory of attention: Time to move on? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[43]  V. Franz,et al.  Action does not resist visual illusions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[44]  David Whitney,et al.  Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  Duje Tadin,et al.  Modularity in the motion system: independent oculomotor and perceptual processing of brief moving stimuli. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[46]  K. D. De Valois,et al.  Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.

[47]  Brent R Beutter,et al.  Human discrimination of visual direction of motion with and without smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[48]  B. Bridgeman,et al.  Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.

[49]  Laurent Perrinet,et al.  Saccadic foveation of a moving visual target in the rhesus monkey. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[51]  Joshua A. Solomon,et al.  Tilted frames of reference have similar effects on the perception of gravitational vertical and the planning of vertical saccadic eye movements , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  M. Goodale,et al.  Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  Ari Z. Zivotofsky,et al.  A dissociation between perception and action in open-loop smooth-pursuit ocular tracking of the Duncker Illusion , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[54]  Miriam Spering,et al.  Acting without seeing: eye movements reveal visual processing without awareness , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.

[55]  E Brenner,et al.  Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[56]  Pascal Mamassian,et al.  More is not always better: adaptive gain control explains dissociation between perception and action , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Frans W Cornelissen,et al.  The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[58]  Ralf Engbert,et al.  Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  Alessandro Moscatelli,et al.  Modeling psychophysical data at the population-level: the generalized linear mixed model. , 2012, Journal of vision.

[60]  Miguel P Eckstein,et al.  Similar Neural Representations of the Target for Saccades and Perception during Search , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[61]  A. Gorea,et al.  On the perceptual/motor dissociation: a review of concepts, theory, experimental paradigms and data interpretations. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.

[62]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[63]  Dario L. Ringach,et al.  When your eyes see more than you do , 2010, Current Biology.