Dissociation between the Perceptual and Saccadic Localization of Moving Objects
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] M. Donk,et al. Awareness of the saccade goal in oculomotor selection: Your eyes go before you know , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[2] R. Gellman,et al. Motion processing for saccadic eye movements in humans , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[3] David A. Robinson,et al. Models of the saccadic eye movement control system , 1973, Kybernetik.
[4] A Mack,et al. Saccadic programming and perceived location. , 1981, Acta psychologica.
[5] Masahiro Takei,et al. Human resource development and visualization , 2009, J. Vis..
[6] M. Morgan,et al. Rapid eye movements to a virtual target are biased by illusory context in the Poggendorff figure , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.
[7] J. McCarley,et al. State-trace analysis of the effects of a visual illusion on saccade amplitudes and perceptual judgments , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[8] David C. Burr,et al. Separate visual representations for perception and action revealed by saccadic eye movements , 2001, Current Biology.
[9] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. The flash grab effect , 2012, Vision Research.
[10] Miriam Spering,et al. Tracking Without Perceiving , 2011, Psychology Science.
[11] Béatrice de Gelder,et al. Collicular Vision Guides Nonconscious Behavior , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[12] P. Sterzer,et al. A direct oculomotor correlate of unconscious visual processing , 2012, Current Biology.
[13] David Whitney,et al. Visual motion shifts saccade targets , 2014, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[14] R. McIntosh,et al. Do we have independent visual streams for perception and action? , 2010, Cognitive neuroscience.
[15] M. Goodale,et al. Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand , 1995, Current Biology.
[16] V. Ramachandran,et al. Illusory Displacement of Equiluminous Kinetic Edges , 1990, Perception.
[17] S. Mateeff,et al. Peripheral vision and perceived visual direction , 1983, Biological Cybernetics.
[18] H. Deubel. ADAPTIVITY OF GAIN AND DIRECTION IN OBLIQUE SACCADES1 , 1987 .
[19] Tirin Moore,et al. Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2007, Neuron.
[20] H. Ono,et al. Saccadic eye movements during changes in fixation to stimuli at different distances , 1977, Vision Research.
[21] Duje Tadin,et al. Unifying account of visual motion and position perception , 2015, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[22] A Straube,et al. Differential effect of a distractor on primary saccades and perceptual localization , 2002, Vision Research.
[23] Alan Cowey,et al. On the usefulness of ‘what’ and ‘where’ pathways in vision , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[24] J. Movshon,et al. Integration of sensory evidence in motion discrimination. , 2007, Journal of vision.
[25] J. V. Gisbergen,et al. Scatter in the metrics of saccades and properties of the collicular motor map , 1989, Vision Research.
[26] Current Biology , 2012, Current Biology.
[27] Patrick Cavanagh,et al. Masking produces compression of space and time in the absence of eye movements. , 2014, Journal of neurophysiology.
[28] M. Goodale,et al. Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.
[29] Leslie Welch,et al. The precision of size constancy , 1992, Vision Research.
[30] Chang-Bing Huang,et al. Transitions between Central and Peripheral Vision Create Spatial/Temporal Distortions: A Hypothesis Concerning the Perceived Break of the Curveball , 2010, PloS one.
[31] David Whitney,et al. The influence of visual motion on perceived position , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[32] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[33] Karl R Gegenfurtner,et al. Contrast and assimilation in motion perception and smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2007, Journal of neurophysiology.
[34] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[35] David Whitney,et al. The Emergence of Perceived Position in the Visual System , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[36] Nicola Bruno,et al. When does action resist visual illusions? , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[37] Dirk Kerzel,et al. Motion-induced illusory displacement reexamined: differences between perception and action? , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.
[38] Maria Concetta Morrone,et al. Visual motion distorts visual and motor space. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[39] Leland S. Stone,et al. Expansion of Direction Space around the Cardinal Axes Revealed by Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements , 2005, Neuron.
[40] P.-J. Hsieh,et al. The infinite regress illusion reveals faulty integration of local and global motion signals , 2006, Vision Research.
[41] David C. Burr,et al. Compression of visual space before saccades , 1997, Nature.
[42] T. Schenk,et al. The Premotor theory of attention: Time to move on? , 2012, Neuropsychologia.
[43] V. Franz,et al. Action does not resist visual illusions , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] David Whitney,et al. Motion distorts visual space: shifting the perceived position of remote stationary objects , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[45] Duje Tadin,et al. Modularity in the motion system: independent oculomotor and perceptual processing of brief moving stimuli. , 2014, Journal of vision.
[46] K. D. De Valois,et al. Vernier acuity with stationary moving Gabors. , 1991, Vision research.
[47] Brent R Beutter,et al. Human discrimination of visual direction of motion with and without smooth pursuit eye movements. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[48] B. Bridgeman,et al. Segregation of cognitive and motor aspects of visual function using induced motion , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[49] Laurent Perrinet,et al. Saccadic foveation of a moving visual target in the rhesus monkey. , 2011, Journal of neurophysiology.
[50] A. Watson,et al. Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.
[51] Joshua A. Solomon,et al. Tilted frames of reference have similar effects on the perception of gravitational vertical and the planning of vertical saccadic eye movements , 2015, Experimental Brain Research.
[52] M. Goodale,et al. Two visual systems re-viewed , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[53] Ari Z. Zivotofsky,et al. A dissociation between perception and action in open-loop smooth-pursuit ocular tracking of the Duncker Illusion , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.
[54] Miriam Spering,et al. Acting without seeing: eye movements reveal visual processing without awareness , 2015, Trends in Neurosciences.
[55] E Brenner,et al. Perception and action are based on the same visual information: distinction between position and velocity. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[56] Pascal Mamassian,et al. More is not always better: adaptive gain control explains dissociation between perception and action , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.
[57] Frans W Cornelissen,et al. The Eyelink Toolbox: Eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox , 2002, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[58] Ralf Engbert,et al. Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[59] Alessandro Moscatelli,et al. Modeling psychophysical data at the population-level: the generalized linear mixed model. , 2012, Journal of vision.
[60] Miguel P Eckstein,et al. Similar Neural Representations of the Target for Saccades and Perception during Search , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[61] A. Gorea,et al. On the perceptual/motor dissociation: a review of concepts, theory, experimental paradigms and data interpretations. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.
[62] P. Cavanagh,et al. Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] Dario L. Ringach,et al. When your eyes see more than you do , 2010, Current Biology.