Basic Math in Monkeys and College Students

Adult humans possess a sophisticated repertoire of mathematical faculties. Many of these capacities are rooted in symbolic language and are therefore unlikely to be shared with nonhuman animals. However, a subset of these skills is shared with other animals, and this set is considered a cognitive vestige of our common evolutionary history. Current evidence indicates that humans and nonhuman animals share a core set of abilities for representing and comparing approximate numerosities nonverbally; however, it remains unclear whether nonhuman animals can perform approximate mental arithmetic. Here we show that monkeys can mentally add the numerical values of two sets of objects and choose a visual array that roughly corresponds to the arithmetic sum of these two sets. Furthermore, monkeys' performance during these calculations adheres to the same pattern as humans tested on the same nonverbal addition task. Our data demonstrate that nonverbal arithmetic is not unique to humans but is instead part of an evolutionarily primitive system for mathematical thinking shared by monkeys.

[1]  R. Church,et al.  A mode control model of counting and timing processes. , 1983, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[2]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Weber's Law influences numerical representations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) , 2006, Animal Cognition.

[3]  M. Hauser,et al.  Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? , 2001, Animal Behaviour.

[4]  ROBERT S. MOYER,et al.  Time required for Judgements of Numerical Inequality , 1967, Nature.

[5]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[6]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  Semantic congruity affects numerical judgments similarly in monkeys and humans. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  Noam Chomsky Language and Problems of Knowledge , 1987 .

[9]  M. Beran,et al.  Chimpanzees Remember the Results of One-by-One Addition of Food Items to Sets Over Extended Time Periods , 2004, Psychological science.

[10]  Jonathan I. Flombaum,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers , 2005, Cognition.

[11]  R. Jaeger,et al.  Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian , 2003, Animal Cognition.

[12]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[13]  Laurie R Santos,et al.  Expectations about numerical events in four lemur species (Eulemur fulvus, Eulemur mongoz, Lemur catta and Varecia rubra) , 2005, Animal Cognition.

[14]  永福 智志 The Organization of Learning , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  S. Dehaene Varieties of numerical abilities , 1992, Cognition.

[16]  E. Brannon What Animals Know about Numbers , 2004 .

[17]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The Effect of Heterogeneity on Numerical Ordering in Rhesus Monkeys , 2006 .

[18]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children , 2006, Cognition.

[19]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Numerical representations in primates. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[20]  N Suga,et al.  Amplitude spectrum representation in the Doppler-shifted-CF processing area of the auditory cortex of the mustache bat. , 1977, Science.

[21]  W A Roberts,et al.  Summation of symbols by pigeons (Columba livia): the importance of number and mass of reward items. , 2000, Journal of comparative psychology.

[22]  C. Packer,et al.  Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo , 1994, Animal Behaviour.

[23]  J. Cantlon,et al.  Shared System for Ordering Small and Large Numbers in Monkeys and Humans , 2006, Psychological science.

[24]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .

[25]  S. Dehaene Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation , 2007 .

[26]  Nancy Kanwisher,et al.  Nonsymbolic Arithmetic in Adults and Young Children , 2003 .

[27]  G. Berntson,et al.  Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). , 1989, Journal of comparative psychology.

[28]  Jacky Emmerton,et al.  Pigeons’ serial ordering of numerosity with visual arrays , 1997 .

[29]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.

[30]  Michael J Beran,et al.  Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) enumerate large and small sequentially presented sets of items using analog numerical representations. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[31]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of mathematics , 2004 .

[32]  Hilary Barth,et al.  Abstract number and arithmetic in preschool children. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[33]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[34]  David A Washburn,et al.  Nonverbal Estimation during Numerosity Judgements by Adult Humans , 2006, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  Michael W. L. Chee,et al.  Neural correlates of symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[36]  Jamie I. D. Campbell Handbook of mathematical cognition , 2004 .

[37]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .