Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] D. Berch,et al. Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. , 1999, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[2] Stephen E. Fienberg,et al. Discrete Multivariate Analysis: Theory and Practice , 1976 .
[3] Anja Ischebeck,et al. On the relative speed account of number-size interference in comparative judgments of numerals. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] W. Schwarz,et al. On the temporal dynamics of digit comparison processes , 1998 .
[5] Mark L. Latash,et al. Mirror Writing: Learning, Transfer, and Implications for Internal Inverse Models. , 1999, Journal of motor behavior.
[6] M. Corballis. Can commissurotomized subjects compare digits between the visual fields? , 1994, Neuropsychologia.
[7] P. Dixon,et al. Dynamic illusion effects in a reaching task: evidence for separate visual representations in the planning and control of reaching. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[8] R. Verleger,et al. Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] M. Goodale,et al. The visual brain in action , 1995 .
[10] M. McCloskey. Cognitive mechanisms in numerical processing: Evidence from acquired dyscalculia , 1992, Cognition.
[11] Brian Butterworth,et al. The Mathematical Brain , 1999 .
[12] S Dehaene,et al. Attention, automaticity, and levels of representation in number processing. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[13] L Girelli,et al. The development of automaticity in accessing number magnitude. , 2000, Journal of experimental child psychology.
[14] Matthew F. S. Rushworth,et al. The Mental Number Line and the Human Angular Gyrus , 2001, NeuroImage.
[15] S. Dehaene,et al. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[16] M. Tlauka. The processing of numbers in choice‐reaction tasks , 2002 .
[17] S. Dehaene,et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. , 1993 .
[18] M. H. Fischer,et al. Number processing induces spatial performance biases , 2001, Neurology.
[19] W. Fias. The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC Effect , 1996 .
[20] Michael D. Dodd,et al. Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[21] Daniel Algom,et al. Comparative judgment of numerosity and numerical magnitude: attention preempts automaticity. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] C. Gallistel,et al. Language and spatial frames of reference in mind and brain , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[23] Avishai Henik,et al. Automatic and intentional processing of numerical information , 1992 .
[24] G. Logan,et al. Magnitude versus parity in numerical judgements: event-related brain potentials implicate response conflict as the source of interference. , 1996, Acta psychologica.
[25] K. Nakayama,et al. Enhanced Perception of Illusory Contours in the Lower Versus Upper Visual Hemifields , 1996, Science.
[26] D. Algom,et al. The perception of number from the separability of the stimulus: The Stroop effect revisited , 1996, Memory & cognition.
[27] S. Dehaene,et al. The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .
[28] W Schwarz,et al. Sequential effects in number comparison. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Peter Dixon,et al. Dynamic effects of the Ebbinghaus illusion in grasping: Support for a planning/control model of action , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[30] R. Schmidt,et al. Motor control and learning: A behavioral emphasis, 4th ed. , 2005 .
[31] Derek Besner,et al. Ideographic and alphabetic processing in skilled reading of English , 1979, Neuropsychologia.
[32] Timothy D. Lee,et al. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis , 1982 .
[33] D. Algom,et al. Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgment of numerals: The role of attention. , 1999 .
[34] P. Holland,et al. Discrete Multivariate Analysis. , 1976 .
[35] W. Fias,et al. Semantic Influences on Feature-Based Attention Due to Overlap of Neural Circuits , 2002, Cortex.
[36] M. Goldberg,et al. Space and attention in parietal cortex. , 1999, Annual review of neuroscience.
[37] W Fias,et al. Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. , 2001, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[38] K. Priftis,et al. Brain damage: Neglect disrupts the mental number line , 2002, Nature.
[39] D. A. Taylor,et al. The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[40] F. Restle. Speed of Adding and Comparing Numbers. , 1970 .
[41] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. The Organization of Brain Activations in Number Comparison: Event-Related Potentials and the Additive-Factors Method , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[42] M. Ashcraft. Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory , 1992, Cognition.
[43] H. Heinze,et al. On the interaction of numerical and size information in digit comparison: a behavioral and event-related potential study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[44] C. Gallistel,et al. Preverbal and verbal counting and computation , 1992, Cognition.
[45] E. Spelke,et al. Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.
[46] Peter Brugger,et al. Stimulus-response compatibility in representational space , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[47] S. Dehaene,et al. Differential Contributions of the Left and Right Inferior Parietal Lobules to Number Processing , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[48] W Fias,et al. Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: evidence from the SNARC effect , 2001, Psychological research.