INTERPERSONAL RELATIONS AND GROUP PROCESSES The Influence of Mortality and Socioeconomic Status on Risk and Delayed Rewards: A Life History Theory Approach

Why do some people take risks and live for the present, whereas others avoid risks and save for the future? The evolutionary framework of life history theory predicts that preferences for risk and delay in gratification should be influenced by mortality and resource scarcity. A series of experiments examined how mortality cues influenced decisions involving risk preference (e.g., $10 for sure vs. 50% chance of $20) and temporal discounting (e.g., $5 now vs. $10 later). The effect of mortality depended critically on whether people grew up in a relatively resource-scarce or resource-plentiful environment. For individuals who grew up relatively poor, mortality cues led them to value the present and gamble for big immediate rewards. Conversely, for individuals who grew up relatively wealthy, mortality cues led them to value the future and avoid risky gambles. Overall, mortality cues appear to propel individuals toward diverging life history strategies as a function of childhood socioeconomic status, suggesting important implications for how environmental factors influence economic decisions and risky behaviors.

[1]  J. Belsky,et al.  Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: and evolutionary theory of socialization. , 1991, Child development.

[2]  S. Stearns,et al.  The Evolution of Life Histories , 1992 .

[3]  A. Falk,et al.  Are Risk Aversion and Impatience Related to Cognitive Ability? , 2007, SSRN Electronic Journal.

[4]  M. Del Giudice,et al.  Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies , 2009, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[5]  Daniel E. Geer,et al.  Risk Aversion , 2012, IEEE Secur. Priv..

[6]  J. Lynch,et al.  Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality in adulthood: systematic review and interpretation. , 2004, Epidemiologic reviews.

[7]  Thomas Eckes,et al.  The developmental social psychology of gender , 2000 .

[8]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Deep Rationality: The Evolutionary Economics of Decision Making. , 2009, Social cognition.

[9]  Stephen C. Steams The influence of size and phylogeny on patterns of covariation among life-history traits in the mammals , 1983 .

[10]  Kim Hill,et al.  LIFE HISTORY TRAITS IN HUMANS: Theory and Empirical Studies , 1999 .

[11]  Roy F. Baumeister,et al.  Public Self and Private Self , 2011 .

[12]  C. Both,et al.  Is clutch size individually optimized , 1999 .

[13]  A. Cavaggioni,et al.  Pheromonally accelerated puberty is enhanced by previous experience of the same stimulus , 1995, Physiology & Behavior.

[14]  An evolutionary life-history model of gender differences and similarities. , 2000 .

[15]  Paul H. Rubin,et al.  AN EVOLUTIONARY MODEL OF TASTE FOR RISK , 1979 .

[16]  D. Cross,et al.  Subjective probability and delay. , 1991, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[17]  Kim Hill,et al.  A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity , 2000 .

[18]  M. Gurven,et al.  The trade-off between number and size of offspring in humans and other primates , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[19]  B. Ellis,et al.  Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.10170S0954579405050145 , 2022 .

[20]  D. Maestripieri,et al.  Early experience affects the intergenerational transmission of infant abuse in rhesus monkeys. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Eva Jablonka,et al.  Transgenerational Epigenetic Inheritance: Prevalence, Mechanisms, and Implications for the Study of Heredity and Evolution , 2009, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[22]  W. J. Jacobs,et al.  Consilience and Life History Theory: From Genes to Brain to Reproductive Strategy. , 2006 .

[23]  George C. Williams,et al.  PLEIOTROPY, NATURAL SELECTION, AND THE EVOLUTION OF SENESCENCE , 1957, Science of Aging Knowledge Environment.

[24]  Paul H. Harvey,et al.  Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals , 1990 .

[25]  C. Simon,et al.  An evolutionary ecological perspective on demographic transitions: Modeling multiple currencies , 2002, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[26]  Steven L. Neuberg,et al.  Functional projection: how fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[27]  Larry E. Toothaker,et al.  Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions , 1991 .

[28]  Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al.  Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology , 2007 .

[29]  M. Daly,et al.  Carpe Diem: Adaptation and Devaluing the Future , 2005, The Quarterly Review of Biology.

[30]  J. Belsky,et al.  Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework. , 2008 .

[31]  N. Stanietsky,et al.  The interaction of TIGIT with PVR and PVRL2 inhibits human NK cell cytotoxicity , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[32]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Blatant benevolence and conspicuous consumption: when romantic motives elicit strategic costly signals. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[33]  Daniel Nettle,et al.  Time perspective, personality and smoking, body mass, and physical activity: an empirical study. , 2009, British journal of health psychology.

[34]  J. Greenberg,et al.  The Causes and Consequences of a Need for Self-Esteem: A Terror Management Theory , 1986 .

[35]  Mark Schaller,et al.  Fear of the Dark: Interactive Effects of Beliefs about Danger and Ambient Darkness on Ethnic Stereotypes , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[36]  Arnold K. Ho,et al.  Fear Extinction to an Out-Group Face: The Role of Target Gender , 2009 .

[37]  W. Schaffer The Application of Optimal Control Theory to the General Life History Problem , 1983, The American Naturalist.

[38]  D. Nettle An evolutionary model of low mood states. , 2009, Journal of theoretical biology.

[39]  J. Krebs,et al.  Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach , 1978 .

[40]  A. Caspi,et al.  Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene , 2003, Science.

[41]  Jay Belsky,et al.  Infant Attachment Security and the Timing of Puberty , 2010, Psychological science.

[42]  J. L. Gittleman,et al.  The Fast‐Slow Continuum in Mammalian Life History: An Empirical Reevaluation , 2007, The American Naturalist.

[43]  C. Worthman,et al.  Life history and the early origins of health differentials , 2005, American journal of human biology : the official journal of the Human Biology Council.

[44]  Sheldon Cohen,et al.  Childhood Socioeconomic Status and Host Resistance to Infectious Illness in Adulthood , 2004, Psychosomatic medicine.

[45]  Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: a longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. , 1999, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[46]  Cecil L. Smith,et al.  AN APPLICATION OF OPTIMAL CONTROL THEORY. , 1969 .

[47]  Phillip R. Shaver,et al.  Handbook of attachment : theory, research, and clinical applications , 1999 .

[48]  Dirk P. Janssen,et al.  Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition , 2007, Psychological science.

[49]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Environmental contingency in life history strategies: the influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on reproductive timing. , 2011, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[50]  L. T. Ross,et al.  The role of future unpredictability in human risk-taking , 1997, Human nature.

[51]  J S Chisholm,et al.  The evolutionary ecology of attachment organization , 1996, Human nature.

[52]  L. Green,et al.  A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[53]  Margo Wilson,et al.  Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? , 2004, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[54]  S. Cole,et al.  Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[55]  R. Wootton The evolution of life histories: Theory and analysis , 1993, Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries.

[56]  B. Ellis Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: an integrated life history approach. , 2004, Psychological bulletin.

[57]  W R Rice,et al.  'Heads I win, tails you lose': testing directional alternative hypotheses in ecological and evolutionary research. , 1994, Trends in ecology & evolution.

[58]  D. Roff Life History, Evolution of , 2001 .

[59]  D. Nettle Dying young and living fast: variation in life history across English neighborhoods , 2010 .

[60]  A. Kacelnik,et al.  Risky Theories—The Effects of Variance on Foraging Decisions , 1996 .

[61]  Arnold K. Ho,et al.  Fear Extinction to an Out-Group Face , 2009, Psychological science.

[62]  A. Caspi,et al.  Influence of Life Stress on Depression: Moderation by a Polymorphism in the 5-HTT Gene , 2003, Science.

[63]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They're Angry) , 2006, Psychological science.

[64]  J. S. Jones,et al.  Living fast and dying young , 1990, Nature.

[65]  Bobbi S. Low,et al.  Influences on Women's Reproductive Lives , 2008 .

[66]  J. Simpson,et al.  Changes in women's mate preferences across the ovulatory cycle. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[67]  J. Belsky Childhood experiences and reproductive strategies , 2007 .

[68]  R. Quinlan Human parental effort and environmental risk , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[69]  Daniel Nettle,et al.  The Paranoid Optimist: An Integrative Evolutionary Model of Cognitive Biases , 2006, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[70]  M. Giudice Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies. , 2009 .

[71]  A. Figueredo,et al.  The heritability of life history strategy: The k‐factor, covitality, and personality , 2004, Social biology.

[72]  R. Nesse,et al.  An evolutionary life-history framework for understanding sex differences in human mortality rates , 2006, Human nature.

[73]  M. Zeidner,et al.  Threat to Life and Risk-Taking Behaviors: A Review of Empirical Findings and Explanatory Models , 2009, Personality and social psychology review : an official journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

[74]  P. C. Lee,et al.  Death, Hope, and Sex: Life-History Theory and the Development of Reproductive Strategies [and Comments and Reply] , 1993, Current Anthropology.

[75]  Chad R. Mortensen,et al.  Going along versus going alone: when fundamental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[76]  W. Thomas Boyce,et al.  Biological Sensitivity to Context , 2008 .

[77]  M Daly,et al.  What happens when the private sector plans hospital services for the NHS: three case studies under the private finance initiative , 1997, BMJ.

[78]  M. Meaney,et al.  Nongenomic transmission across generations of maternal behavior and stress responses in the rat. , 1999, Science.

[79]  Vladas Griskevicius,et al.  Aggress to impress: hostility as an evolved context-dependent strategy. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[80]  Jeff Davis,et al.  A Longitudinal Study of the Effects of Uncertainty on Reproductive Behaviors , 2008, Human nature.

[81]  Sanford L. Braver,et al.  On Splitting the Tails Unequally: a New Perspective on One-Versus Two-Tailed Tests , 1975 .