Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of mathematics

[1]  B. Scholl,et al.  Divide and Conquer , 2004, Psychology Science.

[2]  Susan Carey,et al.  Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: Examinations of content and format , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  Lisa Feigenson,et al.  Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants' manual search , 2003 .

[4]  Fei Xu,et al.  Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.

[5]  Marc D Hauser,et al.  Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[6]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  Noam Chomsky,et al.  The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.

[8]  R. J. Young,et al.  Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? , 2002, Animal Cognition.

[9]  Elizabeth M Brannon,et al.  The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy , 2002, Cognition.

[10]  S. Carey,et al.  The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.

[11]  J. Tanji,et al.  Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey , 2002, Nature.

[12]  H. Barth Numerical cognition in adults : representation and manipulation of nonsymbolic quantities , 2002 .

[13]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability , 2002 .

[14]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  Duane M. Rumbaugh,et al.  "Constructive" enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) on a computerized task , 2001, Animal Cognition.

[16]  S. Carey,et al.  Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.

[17]  M. Beran Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.

[18]  B. Scholl Objects and attention: the state of the art , 2001, Cognition.

[19]  M. Hauser,et al.  Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? , 2001, Cognition.

[20]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Language and number: a bilingual training study , 2001, Cognition.

[21]  Richard N. Aslin,et al.  Segmenting a continuous acoustic speech stream: Serial learning in cotton-top tamarin monkeys , 2001 .

[22]  W. Roberts,et al.  Pigeons Flexibly Time or Count on Cue , 2000, Psychological science.

[23]  Susan Carey,et al.  Spontaneous number representation in semi–free–ranging rhesus monkeys , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[24]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  Macaque monkeys categorize images by their ordinal number , 2000, Nature.

[25]  E. Spelke,et al.  Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.

[26]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Cognition: Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee , 2000, Nature.

[27]  M. Hauser Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think , 2000 .

[28]  H S Terrace,et al.  Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[29]  E. Spelke,et al.  Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.

[30]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Tracking Multiple Items Through Occlusion: Clues to Visual Objecthood , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[31]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .

[32]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  What Counts: How Every Brain is Hardwired for Math , 1999 .

[33]  D. Biro,et al.  NUMERICAL ORDERING IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) : PLANNING, EXECUTING, AND MONITORING , 1999 .

[34]  H S Terrace,et al.  Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.

[35]  S. Shettleworth Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .

[36]  Sarah T. Boysen,et al.  Representation of Quantities by Apes , 1997 .

[37]  P. MacNeilage,et al.  Numerical representations in primates. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[38]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Great Ape Societies: Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use , 1996 .

[39]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .

[40]  Michael C. Corballis,et al.  The generation of generativity: a response to Bloom , 1994, Cognition.

[41]  P. Bloom,et al.  Generativity within language and other cognitive domains , 1994, Cognition.

[42]  Z. Pylyshyn,et al.  Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.

[43]  M. Corballis On the evolution of language and generativity , 1992, Cognition.

[44]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.

[45]  F. Gregory Ashby,et al.  Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? , 1992, Psychological research.

[46]  Karen Wynn,et al.  Children's understanding of counting , 1990, Cognition.

[47]  C. Gallistel The organization of learning , 1990 .

[48]  Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al.  Use of numbers by a chimpanzee , 1985, Nature.

[49]  G. Mandler,et al.  Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[50]  John R. Platt,et al.  Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .