Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: A case study of mathematics
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] B. Scholl,et al. Divide and Conquer , 2004, Psychology Science.
[2] Susan Carey,et al. Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: Examinations of content and format , 2003, Cognitive Psychology.
[3] Lisa Feigenson,et al. Tracking individuals via object-files: evidence from infants' manual search , 2003 .
[4] Fei Xu,et al. Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations , 2003, Cognition.
[5] Marc D Hauser,et al. Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representation of numerical magnitudes by cotton–top tamarins , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[6] Stanislas Dehaene,et al. The neural basis of the Weber–Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[7] Noam Chomsky,et al. The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? , 2002, Science.
[8] R. J. Young,et al. Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? , 2002, Animal Cognition.
[9] Elizabeth M Brannon,et al. The development of ordinal numerical knowledge in infancy , 2002, Cognition.
[10] S. Carey,et al. The Representations Underlying Infants' Choice of More: Object Files Versus Analog Magnitudes , 2002, Psychological science.
[11] J. Tanji,et al. Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey , 2002, Nature.
[12] H. Barth. Numerical cognition in adults : representation and manipulation of nonsymbolic quantities , 2002 .
[13] Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al. The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability , 2002 .
[14] Rochel Gelman,et al. Variability signatures distinguish verbal from nonverbal counting for both large and small numbers , 2001, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[15] Duane M. Rumbaugh,et al. "Constructive" enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) on a computerized task , 2001, Animal Cognition.
[16] S. Carey,et al. Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.
[17] M. Beran. Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). , 2001, Journal of comparative psychology.
[18] B. Scholl. Objects and attention: the state of the art , 2001, Cognition.
[19] M. Hauser,et al. Can rhesus monkeys spontaneously subtract? , 2001, Cognition.
[20] Elizabeth S Spelke,et al. Language and number: a bilingual training study , 2001, Cognition.
[21] Richard N. Aslin,et al. Segmenting a continuous acoustic speech stream: Serial learning in cotton-top tamarin monkeys , 2001 .
[22] W. Roberts,et al. Pigeons Flexibly Time or Count on Cue , 2000, Psychological science.
[23] Susan Carey,et al. Spontaneous number representation in semi–free–ranging rhesus monkeys , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[24] Shaul Hochstein,et al. Macaque monkeys categorize images by their ordinal number , 2000, Nature.
[25] E. Spelke,et al. Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants , 2000, Cognition.
[26] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Cognition: Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee , 2000, Nature.
[27] M. Hauser. Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think , 2000 .
[28] H S Terrace,et al. Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[29] E. Spelke,et al. Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. , 1999, Science.
[30] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking Multiple Items Through Occlusion: Clues to Visual Objecthood , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.
[31] C. Gallistel,et al. Nonverbal Counting in Humans: The Psychophysics of Number Representation , 1999 .
[32] Brian Butterworth,et al. What Counts: How Every Brain is Hardwired for Math , 1999 .
[33] D. Biro,et al. NUMERICAL ORDERING IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) : PLANNING, EXECUTING, AND MONITORING , 1999 .
[34] H S Terrace,et al. Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. , 1998, Science.
[35] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[36] Sarah T. Boysen,et al. Representation of Quantities by Apes , 1997 .
[37] P. MacNeilage,et al. Numerical representations in primates. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[38] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Great Ape Societies: Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use , 1996 .
[39] P. Jusczyk,et al. The head-turn preference procedure for testing auditory perception , 1995 .
[40] Michael C. Corballis,et al. The generation of generativity: a response to Bloom , 1994, Cognition.
[41] P. Bloom,et al. Generativity within language and other cognitive domains , 1994, Cognition.
[42] Z. Pylyshyn,et al. Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited-capacity preattentive stage in vision. , 1994, Psychological review.
[43] M. Corballis. On the evolution of language and generativity , 1992, Cognition.
[44] Karen Wynn,et al. Addition and subtraction by human infants , 1992, Nature.
[45] F. Gregory Ashby,et al. Subitizing: Magical numbers or mere superstition? , 1992, Psychological research.
[46] Karen Wynn,et al. Children's understanding of counting , 1990, Cognition.
[47] C. Gallistel. The organization of learning , 1990 .
[48] Tetsuro Matsuzawa,et al. Use of numbers by a chimpanzee , 1985, Nature.
[49] G. Mandler,et al. Subitizing: an analysis of its component processes. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[50] John R. Platt,et al. Localization of position within a homogeneous behavior chain: Effects of error contingencies , 1971 .