Motor learning by observation: Evidence from a serial reaction time task
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] K. Leibovic. Information Processing in The Nervous System , 1969, Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
[2] B. Fischhoff,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory , 1980 .
[3] K. Heilman,et al. Two forms of ideomotor apraxia , 1982, Neurology.
[4] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[5] H. T. A. Whiting,et al. The Effect of the Availability of a Dynamic Model on the Acquisition of a Complex Cyclical Action , 1987 .
[6] M. Smyth,et al. Movement and Working Memory: Patterns and Positions in Space , 1988, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[7] M. A. Stadler,et al. On learning complex procedural knowledge. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[8] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. On the development of procedural knowledge. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[9] M M Smyth,et al. Working Memory for Movements , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[10] Richard I. Ivry,et al. Attention and structure in sequence learning. , 1990 .
[11] M. Smyth,et al. Space and Movement in Working Memory , 1990, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.
[12] A Bandura,et al. Representational guidance of action production in observational learning: a causal analysis. , 1990, Journal of motor behavior.
[13] Dianne C. Berry,et al. The Role of Action in Implicit Learning , 1991 .
[14] D. Howard,et al. Serial pattern learning by event observation. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[15] R Ivry,et al. Cerebellar Involvement in the Explicit Representation of Temporal Information a , 1993, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[16] M. Jeannerod. The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[17] S. Vogt,et al. On relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. , 1995, British journal of psychology.
[18] K Inomata,et al. Effects of Angle of Model-Demonstration on Learning of Motor Skill , 1995, Perceptual and motor skills.
[19] John Annett,et al. Imagery and motor processes: Editorial overview , 1995 .
[20] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.
[21] U. Mayr,et al. Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning: evidence for independent learning of spatial and nonspatial sequences. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[22] M. Arbib,et al. Language within our grasp , 1998, Trends in Neurosciences.
[23] D R Shanks,et al. Evaluating the relationship between explicit and implicit knowledge in a sequential reaction time task. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[24] L. Proteau,et al. Cognitive Processes Underlying Observational Learning of Motor Skills , 1999 .
[25] J. Mazziotta,et al. Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.
[26] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. Implicit motor sequence learning is not purely perceptual , 1999, Memory & cognition.
[27] N. Harvey,et al. Assessing Sequential Knowledge through Performance Measures: The Influence of Short-term Sequential Effects , 1999 .
[28] R. Hari,et al. Temporal dynamics of cortical representation for action. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[29] A M Burton,et al. Learning complex sequences: no role for observation? , 2001, Psychological research.
[30] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.