Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions.

Prevailing views on how we time the interception of a moving object assume that the visual inputs are informationally sufficient to estimate the time-to-contact from the object's kinematics. Here we present evidence in favor of a different view: the brain makes the best estimate about target motion based on measured kinematics and an a priori guess about the causes of motion. According to this theory, a predictive model is used to extrapolate time-to-contact from expected dynamics (kinetics). We projected a virtual target moving vertically downward on a wide screen with different randomized laws of motion. In the first series of experiments, subjects were asked to intercept this target by punching a real ball that fell hidden behind the screen and arrived in synchrony with the visual target. Subjects systematically timed their motor responses consistent with the assumption of gravity effects on an object's mass, even when the visual target did not accelerate. With training, the gravity model was not switched off but adapted to nonaccelerating targets by shifting the time of motor activation. In the second series of experiments, there was no real ball falling behind the screen. Instead the subjects were required to intercept the visual target by clicking a mousebutton. In this case, subjects timed their responses consistent with the assumption of uniform motion in the absence of forces, even when the target actually accelerated. Overall, the results are in accord with the theory that motor responses evoked by visual kinematics are modulated by a prior of the target dynamics. The prior appears surprisingly resistant to modifications based on performance errors.

[1]  R. Gottsdanker,et al.  Identifying the acceleration of visual targets. , 1961, British journal of psychology.

[2]  S. Runeson Visual prediction of collision with natural and nonnatural motion functions , 1975 .

[3]  R. Christina,et al.  Psychology of motor behavior and sport , 1978 .

[4]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[5]  L. Carlton Processing visual feedback information for movement control. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[6]  David N. Lee,et al.  Plummeting gannets: a paradigm of ecological optics , 1981, Nature.

[7]  David N. Lee,et al.  Visual Timing in Hitting An Accelerating Ball , 1983, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[8]  R. Shepard Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. , 1984, Psychological review.

[9]  L. Kaufman,et al.  Handbook of perception and human performance , 1986 .

[10]  J. Freyd Dynamic mental representations. , 1987, Psychological review.

[11]  P. Viviani,et al.  Visuo-Manual Pursuit Tracking of Human Two-Dimensional Movements , 1987 .

[12]  Bruce Abernethy,et al.  Timing structure in squash strokes: Further evidence for the operational timing hypothesis , 1988 .

[13]  G. Orban,et al.  Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.

[14]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[15]  O J Grüsser,et al.  Localization and responses of neurones in the parieto‐insular vestibular cortex of awake monkeys (Macaca fascicularis). , 1990, The Journal of physiology.

[16]  Martin S Banks,et al.  Gravity as a Monocular Cue for Perception of Absolute Distance and/or Absolute Size , 1992 .

[17]  P. Werkhoven,et al.  Visual processing of optic acceleration , 1992, Vision Research.

[18]  R J Bootsma,et al.  Visual information about time-to-collision between two objects. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Francesco Lacquaniti,et al.  The role of vision in tuning anticipatory motor responses of the limbs , 1993 .

[20]  K. J. Cole,et al.  Memory representations underlying motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  J R Tresilian,et al.  Four Questions of Time to Contact: A Critical Examination of Research on Interceptive Timing , 1993, Perception.

[22]  N. A. Borghese,et al.  Time-varying mechanical behavior of multijointed arm in man. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[23]  F A Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Gorassini,et al.  Catching a ball: contributions of intrinsic muscle stiffness, reflexes, and higher order responses. , 1994, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[25]  D. Signorini,et al.  Neural networks , 1995, The Lancet.

[26]  T. Hubbard,et al.  Environmental invariants in the representation of motion: Implied dynamics and representational momentum, gravity, friction, and centripetal force , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[27]  J. Tresilian,et al.  Perceptual and cognitive processes in time-to-contact estimation: Analysis of prediction-motion and relative judgment tasks , 1995, Perception & psychophysics.

[28]  J. Wann Anticipating arrival: is the tau margin a specious theory? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  John P. Wann,et al.  Anticipating arrival: is the tau margin a specious theory? , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets I. Performance and movement initiation , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[31]  Daeyeol Lee,et al.  Manual interception of moving targets II. On-line control of overlapping submovements , 1997, Experimental Brain Research.

[32]  J R Flanagan,et al.  The Role of Internal Models in Motion Planning and Control: Evidence from Grip Force Adjustments during Movements of Hand-Held Loads , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[33]  D Regan,et al.  Visual factors in hitting and catching. , 1997, Journal of sports sciences.

[34]  R. Bootsma,et al.  On the information-based regulation of movement: What Wann (1996) may want to consider , 1997 .

[35]  David N. Lee Guiding Movement by Coupling Taus , 1998 .

[36]  E. Brenner,et al.  Hitting moving targets Continuous control of the acceleration of the hand on the basis of the target’s velocity , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[37]  D M Wolpert,et al.  Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control , 1998, Neural Networks.

[38]  B. Hess,et al.  Oculomotor control of primary eye position discriminates between translation and tilt. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[39]  Simon K. Rushton,et al.  Weighted combination of size and disparity: a computational model for timing a ball catch , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.

[40]  D M Merfeld,et al.  Humans use internal models to estimate gravity and linear acceleration , 1999, Nature.

[41]  J. Tresilian Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  A. Bastian,et al.  Cerebellar subjects show impaired adaptation of anticipatory EMG during catching. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[43]  D Tweed,et al.  Prediction and compensation by an internal model for back forces during finger opening in an overarm throw. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[44]  Michael F. Land,et al.  From eye movements to actions: how batsmen hit the ball , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[45]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Similarity in the response of smooth pursuit and manual tracking to a change in the direction of target motion. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  Manual tracking in two dimensions. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[47]  Reza Shadmehr,et al.  Learning of action through adaptive combination of motor primitives , 2000, Nature.

[48]  E Brenner,et al.  Hitting moving objects. The dependency of hand velocity on the speed of the target. , 2000, Experimental brain research.

[49]  D. Regan,et al.  Visually guided collision avoidance and collision achievement , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[50]  J. Lackner,et al.  Human orientation and movement control in weightless and artificial gravity environments , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[51]  Apostolos P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Guiding contact by coupling the taus of gaps , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[52]  C. Michaels,et al.  Information and action in punching a falling ball , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[53]  R. Shepard Perceptual-cognitive universals as reflections of the world. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[54]  F. Lacquaniti,et al.  Does the brain model Newton's laws? , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[55]  J. F. Soechting,et al.  The Duncker illusion and eye-hand coordination. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[56]  Alexander Thiele,et al.  Speed skills: measuring the visual speed analyzing properties of primate MT neurons , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[57]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[58]  Prrmon Pms LIii,et al.  FACTORS INFLUENCING VELOCITY CODING IN THE HUMAN VISUAL SYSTEM , 2002 .

[59]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Perception of acceleration with short presentation times: Can acceleration be used in interception? , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.

[60]  A. Georgopoulos Cognitive motor control: spatial and temporal aspects , 2002, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[61]  Apostolos P. Georgopoulos,et al.  Interception of real and apparent motion targets: psychophysics in humans and monkeys , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[62]  Anne-Marie Brouwer,et al.  Hitting moving targets , 1998, Experimental Brain Research.

[63]  R. Gellman,et al.  Control strategies in directing the hand to moving targets , 1992, Experimental Brain Research.

[64]  A. Georgopoulos Catching for real and catching for fun in ecological psychology. Focus on "Internal models of target motion: expected dynamics overrides measured kinematics in timing manual interceptions". , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[65]  A. Georgopoulos,et al.  Neural responses during interception of real and apparent circularly moving stimuli in motor cortex and area 7a. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[66]  A. Saija,et al.  Evaluation of local cerebral glucose utilization and the permeability of the blood-brain barrier in the genetically epilepsy-prone rat , 2005, Experimental Brain Research.