Asymmetric Processing of Numerical and Nonnumerical Magnitudes in the Brain: An fMRI Study

It is well established that, when comparing nonsymbolic magnitudes (e.g., dot arrays), adults can use both numerical (i.e., the number of items) and nonnumerical (density, total surface areas, etc.) magnitudes. It is less clear which of these magnitudes is more salient or processed more automatically. In this fMRI study, we used a nonsymbolic comparison task to ask if different brain areas are responsible for the automatic processing of numerical and nonnumerical magnitudes, when participants were instructed to attend to either the numerical or the nonnumerical magnitudes of the same stimuli. An interaction of task (numerical vs. nonnumerical) and congruity (congruent vs. incongruent) was found in the right TPJ. Specifically, this brain region was more strongly activated during numerical processing when the nonnumerical magnitudes were negatively correlated with numerosity (incongruent trials). In contrast, such an interference effect was not evident during nonnumerical processing when the task-irrelevant numerical magnitude was incongruent. In view of the role of the right TPJ in the control of stimulus-driven attention, we argue that these data demonstrate that the processing of nonnumerical magnitudes is more automatic than that of numerical magnitudes and that, therefore, the influence of numerical and nonnumerical variables on each other is asymmetrical.

[1]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Quantities, Amounts, and the Numerical Core System , 2012, Front. Hum. Neurosci.

[2]  S. Dehaene,et al.  The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. , 1998 .

[3]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  Reflexive and Preparatory Selection and Suppression of Salient Information in the Right and Left Posterior Parietal Cortex , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  Manuela Piazza,et al.  Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations , 2010, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  Avishai Henik,et al.  The Brain Locus of Interaction between Number and Size: A Combined Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Event-related Potential Study , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[6]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The Role of Visual Information in Numerosity Estimation , 2012, PloS one.

[7]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? , 2002, Psychological bulletin.

[8]  Bahador Bahrami,et al.  A Candidate for the Attentional Bottleneck: Set-size Specific Modulation of the Right TPJ during Attentive Enumeration , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Development of Elementary Numerical Abilities: A Neuronal Model , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[10]  Alain Content,et al.  Judgement of discrete and continuous quantity in adults: Number counts! , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[11]  Zhentao Zuo,et al.  Effects of number magnitude and notation at 7T: separating the neural response to small and large, symbolic and nonsymbolic number. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Joonkoo Park,et al.  Rapid and Direct Encoding of Numerosity in the Visual Stream. , 2015, Cerebral cortex.

[13]  Daniel Ansari,et al.  Developmental Specialization in the Right Intraparietal Sulcus for the Abstract Representation of Numerical Magnitude , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Fei Xu,et al.  Linking Visual Attention and Number Processing in the Brain: The Role of the Temporo-parietal Junction in Small and Large Symbolic and Nonsymbolic Number Comparison , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  Justin Halberda,et al.  Developmental change in the acuity of approximate number and area representations. , 2013, Developmental psychology.

[16]  H. Barth,et al.  Judgments of discrete and continuous quantity: An illusory Stroop effect , 2008, Cognition.

[17]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  The interplay between nonsymbolic number and its continuous visual properties. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[18]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[19]  Bert Reynvoet,et al.  Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli , 2011, Behavior research methods.

[21]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  Open questions and a proposal: A critical review of the evidence on infant numerical abilities , 2013, Cognition.

[22]  Brian Butterworth,et al.  Discrete and analogue quantity processing in the parietal lobe: a functional MRI study. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[23]  Avishai Henik,et al.  Are numbers special? The comparison systems of the human brain investigated by fMRI , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[24]  Elizabeth S Spelke,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics of processing nonsymbolic number: An event‐related potential source localization study , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[25]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Exact and Approximate Arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene Group , 2004, Science.

[26]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  Beyond the number domain , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[28]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[29]  Rochel Gelman,et al.  Sometimes area counts more than number , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[30]  Wolfgang Grodd,et al.  Comparison of quantities: core and format-dependent regions as revealed by fMRI. , 2012, Cerebral cortex.

[31]  P. E. Pisa,et al.  Quantity discrimination in felines: a preliminary investigation of the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) , 2009, Journal of Ethology.

[32]  Philippe Pinel,et al.  Distributed and Overlapping Cerebral Representations of Number, Size, and Luminance during Comparative Judgments , 2004, Neuron.

[33]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Representation of number in the brain. , 2009, Annual review of neuroscience.

[34]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Comparing Performance in Discrete and Continuous Comparison Tasks , 2014, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[35]  Kelly S. Mix,et al.  Number Versus Contour Length in Infants' Discrimination of Small Visual Sets , 1999 .

[36]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Magnitude processing in non-symbolic stimuli , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[37]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Nonlinear event‐related responses in fMRI , 1998, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[38]  Marco Zorzi,et al.  Emergence of a 'visual number sense' in hierarchical generative models , 2012, Nature Neuroscience.

[39]  E. Spelke,et al.  Language and Conceptual Development series Core systems of number , 2004 .

[40]  Tali Leibovich,et al.  Numerosity processing is context driven even in the subitizing range: An fMRI study , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  Elizabeth M. Brannon,et al.  The Neural Development of an Abstract Concept of Number , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[42]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  The right temporo-parietal junction contributes to visual feature binding , 2014, NeuroImage.

[43]  D. Burr,et al.  A Visual Sense of Number , 2007, Current Biology.

[44]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.